=Paper= {{Paper |id=Vol-1347/paper04 |storemode=property |title=Words matter more than morphemes: evidence from masked priming with bound-stem stimuli |pdfUrl=https://ceur-ws.org/Vol-1347/paper04.pdf |volume=Vol-1347 |dblpUrl=https://dblp.org/rec/conf/networds/GiraudoV15 }} ==Words matter more than morphemes: evidence from masked priming with bound-stem stimuli== https://ceur-ws.org/Vol-1347/paper04.pdf
Words matter more than morphemes: Evidence from masked priming
                    with bound-stem stimuli

              Hélène Giraudo                                                Madeleine Voga
      Laboratoire CLLE (Equipe ERSS)                              University Paul-Valéry Montpellier III
    CNRS & Université Toulouse Jean Jaurès                      madeleine.voga@univ-montp3.fr
       giraudo@univ-tlse2.fr


                                                                se representations within the mental lexicon is a
                      Abstract                                  matter of ongoing research. Two hypotheses can
                                                                be drawn: according to the first, morphemic units
    Five masked priming studies were carried
                                                                correspond to concrete pieces of words (i.e.,
    in order to shed light on the processing of
                                                                stems and affixes). Complex words are therefore
    bound-stem words (e.g., terr- in terrible).
                                                                processed through a decomposition mechanism
    Both orthographic (e.g., termite) and un-
                                                                that strips off the affix in order to isolate the
    related (e.g., montagne ‘montain’) condi-
                                                                stem. The morphemic nature of the remaining
    tions stand as baselines for controlling
                                                                letters is then verified by the system and access
    morphological effects. The results of the
                                                                to word representations (i.e., word forms coded
    experiments using unrelated word con-
                                                                in the orthographic lexicon) operates via the pre-
    trols suggest that in the particular case of
                                                                activation of their constituent morphemes, i.e.,
    bound-stem words, only genuinely de-
                                                                morphemic representations stand as access units.
    rived word primes (terrible) produce pos-
                                                                This mechanism is exemplified by Taft’s model
    itive effects differing from formal over-
                                                                (1994), the basic principles of which are fol-
    lap effects. Morphological effects are in-
                                                                lowed by many psycholinguistic studies (e.g.,
    terpreted as resulting from both “morce-
                                                                Crepaldi, Rastle & Davis, 2010). Morphemic
    me” and “base-lexeme” activations.
                                                                units are situated between the level of let-
1    Introduction                                               ters/syllables and the word level; consequently,
                                                                they can only be matched to concrete letter clus-
As is broadly admitted, morphologically related                 ters (i.e., bound-stems, free-stems and affixes)
words prime each other in various languages                     that constitute words. This decompositional
(Arabic: Boudelaa & Marslen-Wilson, 2001;                       mechanism is also insensitive to any semantic
English: Rastle, Davis, Marslen-Wilson & Tyler,                 characteristics of words (i.e., transparent vs.
2000; French: Giraudo & Grainger, 2000; Ger-                    opaque morphological formation) or to their lex-
man and Dutch: Drews and Zwitserlood, 1995;                     ical environment (in terms of orthographic
Hebrew: Frost, Deutsch & Forster, 1997) thus                    neighborhood or family size). One of the strong
suggesting the existence of a morphological level               predictions of the decompositional approach is
of processing. This kind of study has used differ-              that morphological priming effects should vary
ent types of materials, words or pseudowords, as                following the ease with which constituent mor-
well as multiple settings: for the masked priming               phemes can be identified/extracted.
technique (Forster & Forster, 2003), widely used                According to the second hypothesis, morphology
to shed light on morphological processes as well                is coded at the interface of word and semantic
as in this study, the distinction can be made be-               representations and corresponds rather to lex-
tween designs using only unrelated controls and                 emes (Aronoff, 1994). Lexeme units are coded at
those using both unrelated and orthograph-                      the interface of the word and the semantic level,
ic/phonological controls, as suggested by Girau-                organizing the lexicon in terms of morphological
do & Grainger (2001) or Pastizzo & Feldman                      families. The recognition of any complex word
(2002).                                                         triggers first the activation of all word forms that
Even though the existence of a morphological                    can match with it; a competition is then engaged
level of processing is unanimously acknowl-                     between the pre-activated forms (forms matching
edged, the exact nature, locus and the role of the-             the input, i.e., those who are morphologically

          Copyright © by the paper’s authors. Copying permitted for private and academic purposes.
In Vito Pirrelli, Claudia Marzi, Marcello Ferro (eds.): Word Structure and Word Usage. Proceedings of the NetWordS Final
                          Conference, Pisa, March 30-April 1, 2015, published at http://ceur-ws.org



                                                           19
related but also those who are only orthograph-             stem. Psycholinguists tested this difference and
ically related) until the right lexical representa-         found that processing for free and bound-stems
tion reaches its recognition threshold (deter-              may differ but both produce significant priming
mined by its surface frequency). During this                effects (Forster & Azuma, 2000; Järvikivi &
competition phase, morphologically related                  Niemi, 2002; Marslen-Wilson, Tyler, Waksler &
words send positive activation to their respective          Older, 1994; Pastizzo & Feldman, 2004). Of
base lexeme, feeding back activation to them.               great importance to our study, Pastizzo and
Morphological priming effects result from this              Feldman (2004) observed that the magnitude of
mechanism of co-activation. Following this su-              facilitation varied following the baseline used in
pralexical approach (Giraudo & Grainger, 2000;              the experiments: equivalent magnitudes of prim-
2001), complex words are not “decomposed”,                  ing for free and bound-stems were obtained rela-
but are able to trigger the activation of their con-        tive to an unrelated baseline; with an orthograph-
stituent morphemes. In this kind of architecture,           ic control however, free-stems produced system-
lexeme units are supposed to be abstract enough             atically greater priming than bound-stems. The
to tolerate variation induced by derivation and             interpretation of this line of research suggests
inflection (i.e., allomorphy, suppletion, phono-            that morphological priming effects are not direct-
logical/ morphological truncation, haplology,               ly constrained by semantic similarity between
verb-noun conversion). In other words, a mor-               prime and target. The second handicap, in terms
phological unit does not necessarily need to sur-           of surface analysis, consists in the difficulty in
face in the real world in order to be coded in              segmenting the word forms into morphemes. At
long-term memory. This organisation, compati-               this point, the two different models presented
ble with recent neuroimaging data (Lévy, Ha-                above give rise to different predictions: accord-
goort, Démonet, 2014), also implies that all mor-           ing to the morpheme-based approach all complex
phemes of a given language are not necessarily              forms (free-stem as well as bound-stem words)
represented within the mental lexicon: units such           are first analyzed in morpheme fragments and
as neologisms, hapaxes and nonce words are not              then access word representation, in other words,
necessarily directly connected with existing                the lexicality of the base doesn’t matter. This
morphological units; bound-stem words could be              approach predicts morphological priming be-
such a case.                                                tween derivations (e.g., virus-viral) as well as
                                                            between the base and its derivation (e.g., vir-
                                                            viral).
2    The study                                              According to the supralexical approach, the
                                                            members of a morphological family are linked
The present paper focuses on the processing of
                                                            together by virtue of their common base at the
bound-stem words by opposition to free-stem
                                                            lexeme level; however, the base of bound-stem
words. For ex., on one hand, the word viral com-
                                                            words is not represented at the word level. In this
posed of the bound-stem vir-, also present in vi-
                                                            case, priming effects between related derived
rus, virulent, virulence, virology and virologist
                                                            words (e.g., virus-viral) are expected but no ef-
and, on the other hand the word singer composed
                                                            fect should be observed using their bound-stems
with the free-stem sing that forms singing, song,
                                                            as primes, the access to the base lexeme being
etc. Both are defined as being morphologically
                                                            conditioned by the prior activation of a word
complex but while it is evident for the standard
                                                            form at the word level.
speaker/reader that the complex word singer de-
                                                            Taft and Kougious (2004) investigated this issue
rives from the root sing, it is less evident to say
                                                            in English through a masked priming experi-
from which root the complex word viral derives.
                                                            ment. They compared both semantically and or-
The morpheme vir-, which does not have any
                                                            thographically related words (e.g., virus-viral) to
clear meaning in English, can be considered as a
                                                            merely orthographically related words (e.g., fu-
bound-stem whereas sing- in singer is a free-
                                                            ture-futile) and, unsurprisingly, found facilitation
stem. From a processing point of view, the vir-
                                                            in the former case but not in the latter. Neverthe-
viral example can be viewed as a case where the
                                                            less, the design of this study is not very informa-
lexical unit is not directly connected to the mor-
                                                            tive with respect to the decomposition issue, giv-
phological unit, by virtue of its twofold handi-
                                                            en that the critical condition examining the effect
cap: the first aspect is semantic interpretability,
                                                            of the bound-stem on its derivations has not been
i.e., derivations composed with a bound-stem
                                                            considered.
could be less interpretable than those with a free-




                                                       20
Our study aims to fill this gap through five visual         word primes (e.g., terrible) produced significant
masked priming experiments with native French               priming effects (33ms), though these conditions
speakers. In this kind of protocol, subjects are            did not significantly differ from the bound-stem
unaware of the presence of the prime, which al-             condition (e.g., terr-) whose effect (18ms) did
lows minimizing strategy use and examining au-              not manage to reach significance.
tomatic processing during the early stages of               Exp. 4 was designed to see if the advantage for
word identification: all five experiments use a             the complex word sharing the same bound-stem
within-priming (Latin square) design, in which              found in exp. 3 holds up to the comparison with
we directly compare the effects of different                non-word primes constructed with the same
primes on the same target. A 57ms prime dura-               bound-stem and an existing suffix. The three
tion) was used and the task was lexical decision.           priming conditions were the following: the mor-
Exp. 1 examined morphological effects induced               phologically related word sharing its bound-stem
by words sharing the same bound-stem, e.g., ter-            with the target, e.g., terrible – terreur; a non-
rible – terreur ‘terrible-terror’ relative to an or-        word made of the same bound-stem and a suffix
thographic control baseline, e.g., termite –                different to that of the target, e.g., terrage –
terreur’ ‘termite-terror’ (where ‘termite’ is a             terreur (where -age corresponds to an existing
monomorphemic word), as well as an unrelated                morpheme); an unrelated control, e.g., montagne
baseline (montagne – terreur ‘mountain-terror’).            – terreur. The statistical analysis of the results
Results show that only truly derived word primes            revealed that only related word primes (e.g., ter-
produce facilitation, relative to unrelated (36ms           rible) produced significant morphological prim-
of facilitation) as well as orthographic controls           ing (40ms) relative to the unrelated controls.
(35ms). However, this first result does not in-             Even if the non-word prime condition (e.g., ter-
form us about how derived words constructed                 rage) led to quicker reaction times compared to
with a bound-stem are processed: are they ana-              the unrelated baseline (688 vs 703ms), it didn’t
lyzed in terms of stem + affix or are they global-          differ significantly from it. More importantly, the
ly processed? Exp. 2 examined the extent to                 25ms difference between the word prime condi-
which the facilitation we take as morphological             tion and the non-word one is statistically signifi-
could be due to formal overlap: this is done by             cant. This suggests that it takes a real word to
using non-existent orthographic controls, sharing           induce morphological priming, independently
all but one letter with the ‘true’ bound-stem, e.g.,        and above orthographic low-level perceptual in-
for the target terreur, the first possible prime is         fluences, to which the masked priming technique
the true bound-stem terr- presented in isolation            is known to be sensitive. Our results show that
(e.g., terr - terreur); the second priming condi-           the presence of an existing bound-stem in a non-
tion is the non-existing bound-stem condition               word does not suffice to produce morphological
(orthographic control) tarr- (e.g., tarr – terreur);        priming, a finding which contradicts those pub-
the third condition is an unrelated baseline (e.g.,         lished by Longtin and Meunier (2005) as we
montag – terreur). Although only true bound-                shall see in the discussion. Experiment 5 exam-
stems induced significant facilitation relative to          ined the extent to which the morphological facili-
the unrelated baseline (28ms), the non-existing             tation found in exp. 4 could be due to formal fac-
stem condition (e.g., tarr-) exhibited reaction             tors: in order to test this, we replaced the mor-
times (RTs) that did not differ significantly from          phologically related word primes by non-words
those of the true bound-stem condition. This re-            constructed with a bound-stem and a final letter
sult highlights the fact, already pointed out by            sequence that does not correspond to any suffix
Forster (1999), that there is an influence of for-          in French. The following three prime conditions
mal factors in this kind of protocol, as well as the        defined the three levels of the prime type factor:
need to include orthographic controls in the de-            a complex non-word formed by a bound-stem
sign. Experiment 3 directly compared the effects            and a suffix, e.g., terrage – terreur (where terr-
of complex word primes to those of bound-stem               and –age correspond to existing morphemes); a
primes: the targets were the same as in Exp. 1              simplex non-word formed by a bound-stem and a
and the three levels of the prime type factor were          non-existing ending, e.g., terryme – terreur, in
the following: a morphologically related suffixed           which –yme is not a suffix; finally, an unrelated
word sharing the same bound-stem, e.g., terrible            non-word, e.g., moitagne – terreur. The statisti-
– terreur ‘terrible-terror’; its bound-stem, e.g.,          cal analysis of the results revealed that both
terr - terreur; an unrelated control, e.g., monta-          complex and simplex non-word primes produced
gne – terreur. Results showed that only complex             shorter RTs than unrelated primes (31 and 27ms




                                                       21
of effect respectively): both types of prime are            Taken together, the results of the experiments
able to facilitate target recognition and produce           using unrelated word controls (exp. 1, 3 and 4)
thus morphological-like facilitation. Neverthe-             suggest that in the particular case of bound-stem
less, the fact that the effects produced by com-            words, only genuinely derived word primes (ter-
plex primes (e.g., terrage) did not differ from             rible) produce positive effects differing from
those produced by simplex non-word primes                   formal overlap effects. This is true with the ex-
(e.g., terryme) leads us to reject any interpreta-          ception of exp. 3, where the effect of genuinely
tion based on pre-lexical morphological decom-              derived word primes did not differ from bound-
position. We suggest interpreting this pattern of           stem primes (terr-); note however that in this
results on the basis of formal criteria: for real           experiment, the bound-stem condition did not
words it takes a real word to facilitate processing         differ from the unrelated condition, while the
(exp. 4), but for non-words, given the absence of           derived word condition did. This is a demonstra-
representation in the word-level, morphological-            tion of the fact that “nonwords would be always
like priming does nothing but reflect low-level             better form-primes than words, even when
perceptual similarities, such as between the two            masked. The reason is simply because a related
non-words (both complex and simplex, ter-                   word prime will compete more vigorously with
rage/terryme) and the target terreur. Besides the           the target than a related nonword prime” (For-
role attributed to formal factors, the point that           ster, 1999: 8). These results are not in accordance
should be stressed in the interpretation of exp. 4          with those found by Longtin and Meunier (2005)
and 5 is that while in exp. 4 the nonword made              using roughly the same priming conditions. In
up from an existing bound-stem and an existing              their study, derived non-word primes (e.g.,
suffix (terrage) seems to interfere with pro-               garagité) systematically produced significant
cessing of the target (terreur) by virtue of its            priming effects on target recognition relative to
morphological structure, in exp. 5 this interfer-           unrelated     word      controls,    while     non-
ence disappears. The fact that, in the ‘terrage’            morphological non-word primes (e.g., rapiduit)
condition (exp. 4) we observe RTs that are not              yielded a 29 ms non-significant effect. Two fac-
significantly quicker than the unrelated condi-             tors can explain these contradictory results: a)
tion, despite the existence of a formal overlap             the type of unrelated controls: contrary to Long-
combined with morphological-like structure                  tin and Meunier, we examined priming effects
(terr-age/terr-eur), can only be due to some kind           relative to unrelated non-word primes when the
of interference, otherwise we should observe at             prime conditions included non-words and word
least a small formal effect. This interference nev-         primes when the prime conditions included
ertheless disappears in exp. 5, since both types of         words; b) the type of word targets: given that our
non-words (with existing suffix, e.g., terrage, as          study focuses on bound-stem words, our targets
well as well as non-existing suffix, i.e., simplex          are mandatorily complex words, and not bare-
non-words such as terryme) lead to significant              bases, as in the Longtin & Meunier study. Bare-
facilitation. We therefore obtain a different pat-          bases are by definition more frequent, and, sub-
tern of priming for words (exp. 4) and for non-             sequently, easier to activate because of their low-
words (exp. 5) which leads us towards an ap-                er activation threshold (due to their residual acti-
proach where lexicality of the prime does matter            vation; for a discussion on this point based on
in the overall pattern of results. Even if the pro-         McClelland & Rumelhart 1981, see Voga & Gi-
cessing system can take advantage of ortho-                 raudo, 2009; Giraudo & Voga, 2014).
graphic similarities between prime and target
(and will not prevent itself from doing so, as exp.
2 showed) this does not tell the whole story, and           3    Discussion: On the representation of
it certainly does not tell a morphological story: it             bound-stem words
is just another demonstration of a fact that re-
searchers working with masked priming are fa-               On the basis of the above results, we can con-
miliar with, namely that this technique is sensi-           clude that recognition of complex words benefits
tive to formal factors (Forster, Mohan & Hector,            from two springs of facilitation: a bottom-up ex-
2003). The experiments presented here provide               citation from a sublexical level and a top-down
evidence that we can use this valuable technique            facilitation from a supralexical level. The idea of
in order to shed light on truly morphological ef-           a double representation for morphology was re-
fects, as opposed to morphological-like effects.            cently expressed by Diependaele, Sandra &
                                                            Grainger (2005), suggesting that the morphologi-




                                                       22
cal level should be situated both above and be-                    Crepaldi, D., Rastle, K., Davis, C. 2010. Morphemes in
low the word-form level. Subsequently, morpho-                        their place: Evidence for position-specific identification
                                                                      of suffixes. Memory & Cognition, 38:312-321.
logical representations would be either defined as                 Drews, E. & Zwitserlood, P. (1995). Morphological and
morphologically constrained orthographic repre-                       orthographic similarity in visual word recognition. Jour-
sentations (depending on frequencies) or as mor-                      nal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception &
phologically constrained semantic representa-                         Performance, 21:1098-1116.
                                                                   Forster, K. I. (1999). The microgenesis of priming effects in
tions (coded in terms of regularities in the map-                     lexical access. Brain and Language, 68:5–15.
ping of word forms onto semantics). In the same                    Forster, K.I. and Azuma, T. 2000 Masked priming for pre-
line, Crepaldi et al. (2010) proposed an extension                    fixed words with bound-stems: Does submit prime per-
of Taft’s (1994) sublexical model integrating a                       mit? Language and Cognitive Processes, 15(4-5):539-
lemma level comprised between an orthographic                         561.
                                                                   Forster, K.I., & Forster, J.C. (2003). DMDX: A Windows
lexicon and the semantic system. However, these                       display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavioral
two models consider the two morphological lev-                        Research Methods: Instruments & Computers, 35:116-
els equivalent, given that they both contain units                    124.
corresponding to concrete morphemes. One may                       Frost, R., Deutch, A., & Forster, K.I. 2000. Decomposing
                                                                      morphologically complex words in a nonlinear morphol-
nevertheless assume that different locations im-                      ogy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
ply different contents: the hybrid model we pro-                      Memory and Cognition, 26:751-765.
pose (Giraudo & Voga, 2014) is based exactly on                    Giraudo, H. & Grainger, J. 2000. Prime word frequency in
this assumption. Within this model, morphologi-                       masked morphological and orthographic priming. Lan-
cal complex words are coded according to two                          guage and Cognitive Processes, 15:421-444.
                                                                   Giraudo, H., & Grainger, J. 2001. Priming complex words:
dimensions, their surface form and their internal                     Evidence for supralexical representation of morphology.
structure. The first level captures the statistical                   Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8(1):127-131.
regularities of morphemes translated in terms of                   Giraudo, H. & Voga, M. (2014). Measuring Morphology:
perceptual saliency in the language. At this level,                   the tip of the iceberg ? A retrospective on 10 years of
                                                                      morphological processing. Carnets de Grammaire,
morphologically complex and pseudo-derived                            22:136-167.
words as well as non-words whose surface struc-                    Järvikivi, J., & Niemi, J. 2002. Form-based representation
ture can be divided into distinct morphemes, are                      in the mental lexicon: Priming (with) bound stem allo-
equally processed. This level is not a morpholog-                     morphs in Finnish. Brain & Language, 81:412-423.
ical level but rather a sub-orthographic level con-                Lévy, J., Hagoort, P., & Démonet, J.F. 2014. A neuronal
                                                                      gamma oscillatory signature during morphological unifi-
taining “morcemes”. The second level, i.e., the                       cation in the left occipitotemporal junction. Human
morphological level is paradigmatically oriented,                     Brain Mapping, 35(12):5847-60.
it deals with the construction of words according                  Longtin, M.-C. & Meunier, F. 2005. Morphological decom-
to morphological rules (Booij, 2005; Corbin,                          position in early visual word processing, Journal of
                                                                      Memory and Language, 53(1), 26-41.
1987/1991); it contains “base-lexemes”, units                      McClelland, J. L., D.E. Rumelhart. 1981. An interactive
abstract enough to tolerate orthographic and                          activation model of context effects in letter perception:
phonological variations produced by derivation                        Part 1. An account of basic findings. Psychological Re-
and inflection processes and connected to their                       view, 88: 375-407.
related word forms on the basis of semantic                        Marslen-Wilson, W. D., Tyler, L. K., Waksler, R., & Older,
                                                                      L. 1994. Morphology and meaning in the English mental
transparency.                                                         lexicon. Psychological Review, 101:3-33.
                                                                   Pastizzo, M. J. and Feldman, L. B. 2004. Morphological
References                                                            processing: A comparison between free and bound-stem
                                                                      facilitation. Brain & Language, 90:31-39.
Aronoff, M. (1994). Morphology by itself. Cambridge: MIT           Rastle, K., Davis, M. H., Marslen-Wilson, W. D., & Tyler,
  Press.                                                              L. K. 2000. Morphological and semantic effects in visual
Boudelaa, S., & Marslen-Wilson, W. D. 2001. Morphologi-               word recognition: A time-course study. Language and
  cal units in the Arabic mental lexicon. Cognition, 81:65-           Cognitive Processes, 15:507-537.
  92.                                                              Taft, M. 1994. Interactive activation as a framework for
Booij, Geert. 2005. Compounding and derivation: evidence              understanding morphological processing. Language &
  for Construction Morphology. In W.U. Dressler, D.                   Cognitive Processes, 9:271-294.
  Kastovsky and F. Rainer (eds) Demarcation in Morphol-            Taft, M., & Kougious, P. 2004. The processing of mor-
  ogy. Amsterdam / Philadelphia: John Benjamins. 109-                 pheme-like units in monomorphemic words. Brain &
  132.                                                                Language, 90:9-16.
Corbin, D. (1987/1991). Morphologie derivationnelle et             Voga, M. & H. Giraudo. 2009. Pseudo-family size influ-
  structuration du lexique, vol. 2. Tubingen/Villeneuve d             ences processing of French inflections: evidence in favor
  ’Ascq: Max Niemeyer Verlag / Presses Universitaires                 of a supralexical account in F. Montermini, G. Boyé, J.
  de Lille.                                                           Tseng (eds), Selected Proceedings of the 6th Décem-
                                                                      brettes: Morphology in Bordeaux. Somerville, MA: Cas-
                                                                      cadilla Proceedings Project, 148-155.




                                                              23