=Paper=
{{Paper
|id=Vol-1347/paper23
|storemode=property
|title=ERP correlates of letter-case in visual word recognition
|pdfUrl=https://ceur-ws.org/Vol-1347/paper23.pdf
|volume=Vol-1347
|dblpUrl=https://dblp.org/rec/conf/networds/FernandezPV15
}}
==ERP correlates of letter-case in visual word recognition==
ERP correlates of letter-case in visual word recognition Barbara Leone-Fernandez Manuel Perea Marta Vergara-Martínez Departamento de Metodo- Departamento de Metodo- Departamento de Psicología logía, Facultad de Psicología logía, Facultad de Psicología Evolutiva, Facultad de Psico- Av. Blasco Ibáñez, 21, Uni- Av. Blasco Ibáñez, 21, Uni- logía versitat de València, 46010, versitat de València, 46010, Av. Blasco Ibáñez, 21, Uni- Spain Spain versitat de València, 46010, barbara.leone@uv.es manuel.perea@uv.es Spain marta.vergara@uv.es of different frequency (high and low). They 1 Introduction found that the N170 amplitude, related to struc- tural encoding, was sensitive to case mixing, but Visual word recognition is a key element of lan- the P3, related to stimulus categorization, was guage comprehension. The vast majority of cur- sensitive to lexicality and word frequency. They rent models assume that the recognition of a proposed that case mixing affects early pro- printed word is based on the activation of ab- cessing stages of visual word recognition. stract letter identity representations. The hierar- chical neural accounts of letter/word recognition The Lien et al. experiment is important, but it of Dehaene, Cohen, Sigman, and Vinckier does not respond to the question of whether let- (2005) and Grainger, Rey, and Dufau (2008) ter-case plays a role during visual-word recogni- posit that, early in the process of lexical access, tion with visually familiar words –note that there are neuronal assemblies that respond to the mIxEd-cAsE stimuli are visually unfamiliar and word’s case-specific letters (e.g., they respond to difficult to process. In contrast, lowercase and ‘e’ but not to ‘E’). Later in processing, there are uppercase words are the usual format when read- neuronal assemblies that respond to the abstract ing words. Indeed, experiments on visual-word representation of the letter identity (e.g., they recognition employ either lowercase or upper- respond to the same degree to ‘e’ and to ‘E’). case words with no explicit justification. Importantly, there is one account that does as- Behavioral evidence using masked priming (i.e., sume that letter-case information may form an a paradigm that taps onto early word processing; integral part of a word’s lexical representation. Forster & Davis, 1984; see Grainger, 2008, for Specifically, Peressotti, Cubelli, and Job (2003) review) has revealed that there is a rapid access claimed that ‘while size, font and style (cursive to case-invariant letter representations. Specifi- or print) affect the visual shape of letters, the up- cally, the advantage of the identity condition percase–lowercase distinction is abstract in na- over the unrelated condition is independent of ture as it is an intrinsic property of letters’ (p. the letter-case (similar advantage for kiss-KISS 108). In the framework of Peressotti et al.’s ‘or- and EDGE-edge; see Bowers, Vigliocco, & thographic cue’ account, a given lexical unit Haan, 1998). Furthermore, response times to would not be retrieved only on the basis of the matched-case identical prime-target pairs letter identity and letter position, but also on the (EDGE-EDGE) are virtually similar as the re- basis of letter-case information. Given that most sponse times to mismatched-case identical printed words are presented in lowercase, this prime-target pairs (edge-EDGE; see Jacobs, should provide an advantage for the processing Grainger, & Ferrand, 1995; Perea, Jiménez, & of lowercase vs. uppercase words (see Mayall & Gómez, 2014). Humphreys, 1996; Perea & Rosa, 2002, for be- havioral evidence of a lowercase advantage in To our knowledge, only a previous experiment visual-word recognition). investigated the temporal processing of letter- case using event-related potentials in an un- The main aim of this study is to examine the time masked paradigm (Lien, Allen, & Crawford, course of letter-case on lexical access. The ERPs 2012). Lien et al. compared the processing of may help to disentangle whether letter case is an lowercase-printed vs mIxEd-cAse-printed words attribute that is only relevant in early perceptual Copyright © by the paper’s authors. Copying permitted for private and academic purposes. In Vito Pirrelli, Claudia Marzi, Marcello Ferro (eds.): Word Structure and Word Usage. Proceedings of the NetWordS Final Conference, Pisa, March 30-April 1, 2015, published at http://ceur-ws.org 106 processing or whether it is also relevant in the retrieval of lexical representations. To attain this In the N/P150, larger negative values were ob- goal, we examined whether the effects of letter- served for lowercase than for uppercase words, case (lowercase vs. UPPERCASE) are modulat- with a central scalp distribution, whereas the ef- ed by word-frequency (a factor that indicates fect of word-frequency was not significant. In the lexical/semantic activation; see Vergara- P200, and only for low-frequency words, larger Martínez, Perea, Gómez, & Swaab, 2013) track- positive values were observed for the lowercase ing the ERP waves in well-studied time windows than for uppercase words in frontal/central scalp (N/P150: 100-170 ms; P200: 170-250 ms; N400: areas. With respect to the N400, the ERP waves 255-450 ms) in a lexical decision task. revealed a dissociation of the letter-case effect for low- and high-frequency words. High- 2 Method frequency words showed an effect of letter-case in an early stage of the N400, whereas low- Twenty-two healthy, right-handed, native Span- frequency words showed an effect of letter-case ish-speaking Valencia University students, naïve (in the opposite direction; see Figure 1) in a later to the manipulation of the stimuli, participated in stage of the N400. the study in exchange for a small gift. We selected a set of 160 words from the Web- accessible EsPal database (Duchon, Perea, Se- bastián-Gallés, Martí, & Carreiras, 2013). Half of the words were of high frequency and half were of low frequency. The two groups of words were matched in relevant psycholinguistic factors (length, orthographic neighborhood, concrete- ness, imageability…). Half of the words were presented in uppercase and half in lowercase (MOTHER; mother). In addition, a list of 160 pseudowords (half in lowercase, half in upper- case) was included for the purposes of the lexical decision task. Participants were instructed to decide as accu- rately and rapidly as possible whether or not the As expected, there was an early pre-lexical effect stimulus was a Spanish word. They pressed one of letter-case that did not interact with word- of two response buttons (YES/NO). The electro- frequency. Importantly, we found an interaction encephalogram (EEG) was recorded from 29 between letter-case and word-frequency not only electrodes, averaged separately for each of the in the N400 time window –which is commonly experimental conditions, each of the subjects and associated to lexical-semantic processing, but each of the electrode sites. For each time win- also the P200 time window, thus supporting the dow, we conducted ANOVAs with word- hypothesis that letter-case may affect the map- frequency (high, low), case (lowercase, UPPER- ping of visual-orthographic information onto CASE), and AP (anterior, central-anterior, cen- word representations. Taken together, the present tral, central-posterior and posterior) as factors in ERP data provide empirical support to the hy- the design. pothesis that letter-case information may be stored in the abstract word representations (Per- Results and Conclusions essotti et al., 2003), thus posing some problems The behavioral data revealed significantly faster for current computational and neural models of responses for high-frequency than for low- visual-word recognition. frequency words (656 vs. 702 ms) and signifi- cantly faster responses for lowercase than for “Figure 1. Grand average ERP waves to Fre- uppercase words (675 vs. 683 ms). There were quency and case manipulations in one repre- no signs of an interaction between the two fac- sentative electrode. Different columns mark the tors. The error data revealed the same pattern as four epochs under analysis” the response time data. 107 References letin & Review, 19:677–684. doi: 10.3758/s13423- 012-0251-9 Andrew Duchon, Manuel Perea, Nuria Sebastián- Gallés, Antonia Martí and Manuel Carreiras. 2013. Stanislas Dehaene, Laurent Cohen, Mariano Sigman EsPal: One-stop shopping for Spanish word prop- and Fabien Vinckier. 2005. The neural code for erties. Behavior Research Methods, 45:1246–1258 written words: A proposal. Trends in Cognitive doi:10.3758/s13428-013-0326-1 Sciences, 9:335–341. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.05.004 Arthur M. Jacobs, Jonathan Grainger and Ludovic Ferrand. 1995. The incremental priming technique: A method for determining within-condition prim- ing effects, Perception & Psychophysics, 57:1101– 1110. doi:10.3758/BF03208367 Francesca Peressotti, Roberto Cubelli and Remo Job. 2003. On recognizing proper names: The ortho- graphic cue hypothesis. Cognitive Psychology, 47:87–116. doi:10.1016/S0010-0285(03)00004-5 Jeffrey S. Bowers, Gabriella Vigliocco and Richard Haan. 1998. Orthographic, phonological, and artic- ulatory contributions to masked letter and word priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24:1705– 1719. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.24.6.1705 Jonathan Grainger, Arnaud Rey and Stéphane Dufau. 2008. Letter perception: from pixels to pandemoni- um. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12:381-387. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2008.06.006 Kate Mayall and Glyn W. Humphreys. 1996. Case mixing and the task-sensitive disruption of lexical processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22:278–294. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.22.2.278 Kenneth I. Forster and Chris Davis. 1984. Repetition priming and frequency attenuation in lexical ac- cess. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learn- ing, Memory, & Cognition, 10:680–698. doi:10/1037/0278-7393.10.4.680 Manuel Perea and Eva Rosa. 2002. Does “whole word shape” play a role in visual word recognition? Per- ception and Psychophysics, 64:785–794. doi:10.3758/BF03194745 Manuel Perea, Maria Jiménez and Pablo Gomez. 2014. A challenging dissociation in masked identi- ty priming with the lexical decision task. Acta Psychologica, 148:130–135. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.01.014 Marta Vergara-Martínez, Manuel Perea, Pablo Gómez and Tamara Y. Swaab. 2013. ERP correlates of let- ter identity and letter position are modulated by lexical-frequency. Brain and Language, 125: 11– 27. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2012.12.009 Mei-Ching Lien, Philip A. Allen and Caitlin Craw- ford. 2012. Electrophysiological evidence of dif- ferent loci for case-mixing and word-frequency ef- fects in visual word recognition. Psychonomic Bul- 108