=Paper= {{Paper |id=Vol-1347/paper30 |storemode=property |title=Language proficiency moderates morphological priming in children and adults |pdfUrl=https://ceur-ws.org/Vol-1347/paper30.pdf |volume=Vol-1347 |dblpUrl=https://dblp.org/rec/conf/networds/HasenackerBS15 }} ==Language proficiency moderates morphological priming in children and adults== https://ceur-ws.org/Vol-1347/paper30.pdf
    Language proficiency moderates morphological priming in children
                              and adults


        Jana Hasenäcker                 Elisabeth Beyersmann                        Sascha Schroeder
      Max-Planck-Institute             Laboratoire de Psychologie                 Max-Planck-Institute
    for Human Development,              Cognitive, Aix-Marseille                for Human Development,
             Berlin                    Université and Centre de la                       Berlin
    hasenaecker@mpib-                   Recherche Scientifique,               sascha.schroeder@mpib-
       berlin.mpg.de                           Marseille                           berlin.mpg.de
                                          lisi.beyersmann
                                            @gmail.com



                                                                   Only few studies have been concerned with
1     Introduction                                            morphological decomposition in beginning read-
                                                              ers. The few studies from English and French
A number of studies have shown that skilled
                                                              used complex word primes, pseudocomplex
readers decompose morphologically complex
                                                              word primes and non-morphological word
words upon encountering them (for a review, see
                                                              primes. Quémart, Casalis and Colé (2011) found
Rastle & Davis, 2008). It has been proposed that
                                                              priming in French grade 3, 5, and 7 children
this segmentation process is early and automatic
                                                              from complex as well as pseudocomplex words,
and is driven by orthographic form, while being
                                                              thus suggesting that children already use adult-
blind to semantic content, thus also called mor-
                                                              like decomposition processes. In contrast, Bey-
pho-orthographic (Rastle, Davis, & New, 2004;
                                                              ersmann, Castles and Coltheart (2012) only
Taft, 2003). One key finding in favor of this
                                                              found priming from truly complex words in
proposition comes from masked morphological
                                                              grade 3 and 5 English-speaking children, indicat-
priming: the recognition of a target word is fa-
                                                              ing that morpho-orthographic priming is not au-
cilitated when it is preceded by a morphological-
                                                              tomatized yet and decomposition relies more on
ly related word prime (teacher-TEACH). Facili-
                                                              semantics in developing readers. However, no
tation has also been found in a number of lan-
                                                              studies with children have used complex
guages for targets preceded by pseudocomplex
                                                              pseudoword primes so far, although they provide
word primes that is words that appear to have a
                                                              the possibility to utilize the paradigm in lan-
morphologically complex structure, but are sim-
                                                              guages that do not naturally have pseudocomplex
plex words (corner-CORN). Moreover, facilita-
                                                              words, such as German.
tion has as well been observed from complex
                                                                   Morphological decomposition in German
pseudoword primes, that is a non-existing com-
                                                              can be insightful to investigate, because of its
bination of a stem and affix (flexify-FLEX). For
                                                              language specific characteristics. German has a
non-morphological nonword primes, that is a
                                                              transparent orthography and is morphologically
non-existing combination of a word and a non-
                                                              rich. As a consequence, morphological entities
morphemic ending (flexint-FLEX), mixed results
                                                              might present a very useful unit for effective
have been obtained (Longtin & Meunier, 2005;
                                                              word recognition, even for beginning readers.
Morris, Porter, Grainger & Holcomb, 2011). Re-
                                                              Nevertheless, for children being still in the pro-
cent evidence from French points to a moderat-
                                                              cess of reading acquisition and showing more
ing role of language proficiency: the magnitude
                                                              variability in their lexical representations, lan-
to which morpho-orthographic information is
                                                              guage proficiency can be expected to play an
used increases as a function of individual vocab-
                                                              even greater role than Beyersmann et al. (2014)
ulary and spelling skills in adults (Andrews &
                                                              found for adults.
Lo, 2013; Beyersmann, Casalis, Ziegler &
                                                                       The aim of the present study was there-
Grainger, 2014).
                                                              fore to test whether the moderating effect of lan-

                Copyright © by the paper’s authors. Copying permitted for private and academic purposes.
In Vito Pirrelli, Claudia Marzi, Marcello Ferro (eds.): Word Structure and Word Usage. Proceedings of the NetWordS Final
                          Conference, Pisa, March 30-April 1, 2015, published at http://ceur-ws.org

                                                          132
guage proficiency, as indexed by vocabulary and         matched on length. For each target all four types
spelling skills, on morphological priming can be        of primes were created. Primes were matched on
replicated with German adults and whether it            length, suffix length and non-morphemic ending
generalizes to readers at the lower end of the pro-     length across conditions. Four counterbalanced
ficiency range, namely children. We expect to           lists of prime-target combinations were created,
see evidence for a more automatized form of             each containing a target only once, such that par-
morpho-orthographic decomposition in highly             ticipants saw each target only with one of the
proficient children (replicating the Quémart et al.     four prime conditions.
pattern), whereas low-skilled children should
show less priming (as in Beyersmann et al.,             2.3   Procedure
2012) or no robust priming at all. In our adult         Stimuli were presented in white 20-point Courier
group, we expect robust priming in all three            New font in the center of a black screen on a 15″
prime conditions (including the nonsuffixed con-        laptop monitor with a refresh rate of 60 Hz. Each
dition) in high proficiency participants, but re-       trial consisted of a 500-ms fixation cross, fol-
duced non-suffixed priming in low proficiency           lowed by a 500-ms forward mask of hash keys,
participants                                            then a prime in lowercase for 50 ms, followed by
                                                        the target in uppercase. The target remained on
                                                        screen until response. Participants were instruct-
2     Method                                            ed to indicate whether the presented stimuli was
2.1    Participants                                     an existing German word or not by pressing a
                                                        key as quickly and as accurately as possible.
Twenty-four university students (13 women, Mage         They were not informed about the presentation
= 25.2 years, age range: 20–29 years) and 24 el-        of the prime.
ementary school children (13 girls, Mage = 9.5
years, age range: 8;6–10;9 years, grade 3-5) par-       2.4   Results
ticipated in the experiment.                            Reaction times were analyzed using linear
         Each participant’s language proficiency        mixed-effects modeling. Participants and items
was assessed, using a spelling and a vocabulary         were included as random factors and lexical sta-
test. Adults performed a spelling recognition test,     tus of the target (word, pseudoword), prime type
which was modelled after the one used by An-            (suffixed word, suffixed pseudoword, nonsuf-
drews and Lo (2012). Participants were asked to         fixed pseudoword, unrelated word), age group
classify 100 words as correctly or incorrectly          (adults, children) and language proficiency (con-
spelled. Children performed the fill-in-the-gap         tinuous measure combined of the spelling and
dictation test of the SLRT-II (Moll & Landerl,          vocabulary scores), as well as all their interac-
2010). For assessment of vocabulary, adults             tions, were included as fixed effects. Where ap-
completed the German version of the LexTALE             propriate, one-sided post-hoc contrasts were ap-
(Lemhöfer & Broersma, 2012), and children the           plied comparing all related priming conditions
vocabulary subtest of the CFT 20 (Weiß, 1998).          with the unrelated condition. For contrasting
A composite measure of spelling and vocabulary          readers with higher and lower proficiency, reac-
was calculated by standardizing and averaging           tion times of participants scoring one standard
the spelling and vocabulary scores for each par-        deviation above or below the mean proficiency
ticipant.                                               measure within their age group were used. Sig-
2.2    Materials                                        nificance was evaluated using the normal distri-
                                                        bution. Results are reported for word targets on-
We conducted a masked priming lexical decision          ly. Descriptive statistics are provided in Table 1.
experiment using real suffixed words (kleidchen-             For adults, priming was observed from all
KLEID), suffixed pseudowords (kleidtum-                 three related conditions (suffixed word, suffixed
KLEID), nonsuffixed pseudowords (kleidekt-              pseudoword and nonsuffixed pseudoword) rela-
KLEID) and unrelated controls (träumerei-               tive to the unrelated condition, z=5.04, z=4.43,
KLEID) as primes. 50 word targets were selected         z=2.07, all p<.05. However, language proficien-
from the childLex corpus (Schroeder, Würzner,           cy moderated priming effects. Priming in the
Heister, Geyken, & Kliegl, 2014) and 50                 nonsuffixed pseudoword condition was only sig-
pseudoword targets were created by changing             nificant for adults with higher language profi-
one letter from a real word that was not in the         ciency (+1SD), z=1.74, p<.05, but not for adults
target word set. Word and nonword targets were




                                                      133
                                                   RT
            Prime Type              Adults                 Children          Stimulus Example
                                             All participants
       Suffixed Word               593 (12)                1024 (36)       kleidchen - KLEID
       Suffixed Nonword            597 (12)                1051 (38)       kleidtum - KLEID
       Nonsuffixed Nonword         614 (13)                1045 (38)       kleidekt - KLEID
       Unrelated                   629 (14)                1087 (41)       träumerei - KLEID
                                Higher Language Proficiency (+1SD)
       Suffixed Word               588 (12)                 900 (28)       kleidchen - KLEID
       Suffixed Nonword            583 (12)                 924 (30)       kleidtum - KLEID
       Nonsuffixed Nonword         602 (12)                 903 (28)       kleidekt - KLEID
       Unrelated                   620 (13)                 974 (33)       träumerei - KLEID
                                Lower Language Proficiency (-1SD)
       Suffixed Word               599 (12)                1189 (48)       kleidchen - KLEID
       Suffixed Nonword            613 (13)                1218 (51)       kleidtum - KLEID
       Nonsuffixed Nonword         626 (14)                1239 (52)       kleidekt - KLEID
       Unrelated                   638 (14)                1229 (51)       träumerei - KLEID

                      Table 1. Response times (in ms) for children and adults,
       averaged across items for each participant. Standard errors are presented in parentheses.


with lower language proficiency (-1SD), z=1.16,         Moreover, the pattern of priming generalizes to
p=.25.                                                  beginning readers with higher language profi-
     For children, proficiency played an even           ciency: they show priming similar to that of
more pronounced role than for adults: higher            higher proficient adults. For children with lower
proficiency children (+1SD) showed the same             language proficiency, the effects did not reach
pattern as higher proficiency adults, namely            significance, but were clearly most pronounced
priming from all related condition, z=3.03,             in the suffixed word condition.
z=2.02, z=2.96, all p<.05. In contrast, in lower                 We argue that there is a developmental
proficiency children (-1SD) priming in none of          gradient in the use of morphological information
the conditions reached significance, although           during reading acquisition, driven by language
there was a numerical advantage from suffixed           proficiency. Beginning readers with low lan-
word primes in the mean reaction times (40ms            guage proficiency are only able to benefit from
faster compared to the unrelated condition).            morpho-semantic information, if at all. More
                                                        advanced lexical knowledge allows readers to
3   Conclusion                                          extract morpho-orthographic information. Fol-
                                                        lowing Andrews and Davis (1999) and Grainger
Our results confirm recent evidence for French
                                                        and Ziegler (2011), we assume that this happens
adults (Beyersmann et al., 2014), showing that
                                                        through segmentation of the affix in lower profi-
the extent to which morphological information is
                                                        ciency adults, as indicated by the priming effects
exploited depends on language proficiency also
                                                        of both suffixed prime conditions. Crucially,
in German. Adults in the present study showed
                                                        higher proficiency adult and even child readers
morphological priming effects from suffixed
                                                        with sophisticated lexical knowledge are able to
word primes (kleidchen-KLEID), suffixed
                                                        additionally use segmentation of the embedded
pseudoword primes (kleidtum-KLEID) and also
                                                        stem, therefore showing facilitation also in the
nonsuffixed pseudoword primes (kleidekt-
                                                        nonsuffixed prime condition. Our results high-
KLEID) relative to unrelated words (träumerei-
                                                        light the importance of lexical knowledge as a
KLEID). Priming from the nonsuffixed
                                                        further determinant of the ability to exploit mor-
pseudoword condition did not continue to be sig-
nificant with decreasing language proficiency.




                                                   134
phological structure in the word recognition pro-                 orthographic segmentation in visual word recogni-
cess, especially in children.                                     tion. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 1090-
                                                                  1098.
Reference
                                                              Schroeder, S., Würzner, K.-M., Heister, J., Geyken,
Andrews, S., & Davis, M. H. (1999). Interactive acti-            A., & Kliegl, R. (2014). childLex: A lexical data-
  vation accounts of morphological decomposition:                base for German read by children. Manuscript
  Finding the trap in mousetrap? Brain and Lan-                  submitted for publication. Max Planck Institute for
  guage, 68, 355-361.                                            Human Development, Berlin.
Andrews, S., & Lo, S. (2013). Is morphological prim-
                                                              Taft, M. (2003). Morphological representation as a
  ing stronger for transparent than opaque words? It
                                                                 correlation between from and meaning. In E. As-
  depends on individual differences in spelling and
                                                                 sink & D. Sandra (Eds.) Reading complex words
  vocabulary. Journal of Memory and Language, 68,
                                                                 (pp. 113-137). Amsterdam, The Netherlands:
  279-296.
                                                                 Kluwer.
Beyersmann, E., Casalis, S., Ziegler, J. C., & Grain-
  ger, J. (2014). Language proficiency and morpho-            Weiß, R. H. (1998). Grundintelligenztest Skala 2
  orthographic segmentation. Psychonomic Bulletin               (CFT 20) mit Wortschatztest (WS) und Zahlenfol-
  Review.                                                       gentest (ZF). Göttingen: Hogrefe.
Beyersmann, E., Castles, A., & Coltheart, M. (2012).
   Morphological processing during visual word
   recognition in developing readers: Evidence from
   masked priming. The Quarterly Journal of Exper-
   imental Psychology, 65, 1306-1326.

Grainger, J., & Ziegler, J. C. (2011). A dual-route
   approach to orthographic processing. Frontiers in
   Psychology, 45,

Lemhöfer, K., & Broersma, M. (2012). Introducing
  LexTALE: A quick and valid Lexical Test for Ad-
  vanced Learners of English. Behavior Research
  Methods, 44, 325-343.

Longtin, C. M., & Meunier, F. (2015). Morphological
  decomposition in early visual word processing.
  Journal of Memory and Language, 53, 26-41.
Moll, K., & Landerl, K. (2010). SLRT-II: Lese- und
  Rechtschreibtest: Weiterentwicklung der Salzbur-
  ger Lese- und Rechtschreibtests (SLRT): Manual.
  Bern: Verlag Hans Huber.

Morris, J., Porter, J. H., Grainger, J., & Holcomb, P. J.
 (2011). Effects of lexical status and morphological
 complexity in masked priming: An ERP study.
 Language & Cognitive Processes, 26, 558-599.
Quémart, P., Casalis, S., & Colé, P. (2011). The role
  of form and meaning in the processing of written
  morphology: A priming study in French develop-
  ing readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psy-
  chology, 109, 478-496.

Rastle, K., & Davis, M. H. (2008). Morphological
  decomposition based on the analysis of orthogra-
  phy. Language & Cognitive Processes, 23, 942-
  971.
Rastle, K., Davis, M. H., & New, B. (2004). The
   broth in my brother’s brothel: Morpho-




                                                            135