=Paper=
{{Paper
|id=Vol-1347/paper30
|storemode=property
|title=Language proficiency moderates morphological priming in children and adults
|pdfUrl=https://ceur-ws.org/Vol-1347/paper30.pdf
|volume=Vol-1347
|dblpUrl=https://dblp.org/rec/conf/networds/HasenackerBS15
}}
==Language proficiency moderates morphological priming in children and adults==
Language proficiency moderates morphological priming in children and adults Jana Hasenäcker Elisabeth Beyersmann Sascha Schroeder Max-Planck-Institute Laboratoire de Psychologie Max-Planck-Institute for Human Development, Cognitive, Aix-Marseille for Human Development, Berlin Université and Centre de la Berlin hasenaecker@mpib- Recherche Scientifique, sascha.schroeder@mpib- berlin.mpg.de Marseille berlin.mpg.de lisi.beyersmann @gmail.com Only few studies have been concerned with 1 Introduction morphological decomposition in beginning read- ers. The few studies from English and French A number of studies have shown that skilled used complex word primes, pseudocomplex readers decompose morphologically complex word primes and non-morphological word words upon encountering them (for a review, see primes. Quémart, Casalis and Colé (2011) found Rastle & Davis, 2008). It has been proposed that priming in French grade 3, 5, and 7 children this segmentation process is early and automatic from complex as well as pseudocomplex words, and is driven by orthographic form, while being thus suggesting that children already use adult- blind to semantic content, thus also called mor- like decomposition processes. In contrast, Bey- pho-orthographic (Rastle, Davis, & New, 2004; ersmann, Castles and Coltheart (2012) only Taft, 2003). One key finding in favor of this found priming from truly complex words in proposition comes from masked morphological grade 3 and 5 English-speaking children, indicat- priming: the recognition of a target word is fa- ing that morpho-orthographic priming is not au- cilitated when it is preceded by a morphological- tomatized yet and decomposition relies more on ly related word prime (teacher-TEACH). Facili- semantics in developing readers. However, no tation has also been found in a number of lan- studies with children have used complex guages for targets preceded by pseudocomplex pseudoword primes so far, although they provide word primes that is words that appear to have a the possibility to utilize the paradigm in lan- morphologically complex structure, but are sim- guages that do not naturally have pseudocomplex plex words (corner-CORN). Moreover, facilita- words, such as German. tion has as well been observed from complex Morphological decomposition in German pseudoword primes, that is a non-existing com- can be insightful to investigate, because of its bination of a stem and affix (flexify-FLEX). For language specific characteristics. German has a non-morphological nonword primes, that is a transparent orthography and is morphologically non-existing combination of a word and a non- rich. As a consequence, morphological entities morphemic ending (flexint-FLEX), mixed results might present a very useful unit for effective have been obtained (Longtin & Meunier, 2005; word recognition, even for beginning readers. Morris, Porter, Grainger & Holcomb, 2011). Re- Nevertheless, for children being still in the pro- cent evidence from French points to a moderat- cess of reading acquisition and showing more ing role of language proficiency: the magnitude variability in their lexical representations, lan- to which morpho-orthographic information is guage proficiency can be expected to play an used increases as a function of individual vocab- even greater role than Beyersmann et al. (2014) ulary and spelling skills in adults (Andrews & found for adults. Lo, 2013; Beyersmann, Casalis, Ziegler & The aim of the present study was there- Grainger, 2014). fore to test whether the moderating effect of lan- Copyright © by the paper’s authors. Copying permitted for private and academic purposes. In Vito Pirrelli, Claudia Marzi, Marcello Ferro (eds.): Word Structure and Word Usage. Proceedings of the NetWordS Final Conference, Pisa, March 30-April 1, 2015, published at http://ceur-ws.org 132 guage proficiency, as indexed by vocabulary and matched on length. For each target all four types spelling skills, on morphological priming can be of primes were created. Primes were matched on replicated with German adults and whether it length, suffix length and non-morphemic ending generalizes to readers at the lower end of the pro- length across conditions. Four counterbalanced ficiency range, namely children. We expect to lists of prime-target combinations were created, see evidence for a more automatized form of each containing a target only once, such that par- morpho-orthographic decomposition in highly ticipants saw each target only with one of the proficient children (replicating the Quémart et al. four prime conditions. pattern), whereas low-skilled children should show less priming (as in Beyersmann et al., 2.3 Procedure 2012) or no robust priming at all. In our adult Stimuli were presented in white 20-point Courier group, we expect robust priming in all three New font in the center of a black screen on a 15″ prime conditions (including the nonsuffixed con- laptop monitor with a refresh rate of 60 Hz. Each dition) in high proficiency participants, but re- trial consisted of a 500-ms fixation cross, fol- duced non-suffixed priming in low proficiency lowed by a 500-ms forward mask of hash keys, participants then a prime in lowercase for 50 ms, followed by the target in uppercase. The target remained on screen until response. Participants were instruct- 2 Method ed to indicate whether the presented stimuli was 2.1 Participants an existing German word or not by pressing a key as quickly and as accurately as possible. Twenty-four university students (13 women, Mage They were not informed about the presentation = 25.2 years, age range: 20–29 years) and 24 el- of the prime. ementary school children (13 girls, Mage = 9.5 years, age range: 8;6–10;9 years, grade 3-5) par- 2.4 Results ticipated in the experiment. Reaction times were analyzed using linear Each participant’s language proficiency mixed-effects modeling. Participants and items was assessed, using a spelling and a vocabulary were included as random factors and lexical sta- test. Adults performed a spelling recognition test, tus of the target (word, pseudoword), prime type which was modelled after the one used by An- (suffixed word, suffixed pseudoword, nonsuf- drews and Lo (2012). Participants were asked to fixed pseudoword, unrelated word), age group classify 100 words as correctly or incorrectly (adults, children) and language proficiency (con- spelled. Children performed the fill-in-the-gap tinuous measure combined of the spelling and dictation test of the SLRT-II (Moll & Landerl, vocabulary scores), as well as all their interac- 2010). For assessment of vocabulary, adults tions, were included as fixed effects. Where ap- completed the German version of the LexTALE propriate, one-sided post-hoc contrasts were ap- (Lemhöfer & Broersma, 2012), and children the plied comparing all related priming conditions vocabulary subtest of the CFT 20 (Weiß, 1998). with the unrelated condition. For contrasting A composite measure of spelling and vocabulary readers with higher and lower proficiency, reac- was calculated by standardizing and averaging tion times of participants scoring one standard the spelling and vocabulary scores for each par- deviation above or below the mean proficiency ticipant. measure within their age group were used. Sig- 2.2 Materials nificance was evaluated using the normal distri- bution. Results are reported for word targets on- We conducted a masked priming lexical decision ly. Descriptive statistics are provided in Table 1. experiment using real suffixed words (kleidchen- For adults, priming was observed from all KLEID), suffixed pseudowords (kleidtum- three related conditions (suffixed word, suffixed KLEID), nonsuffixed pseudowords (kleidekt- pseudoword and nonsuffixed pseudoword) rela- KLEID) and unrelated controls (träumerei- tive to the unrelated condition, z=5.04, z=4.43, KLEID) as primes. 50 word targets were selected z=2.07, all p<.05. However, language proficien- from the childLex corpus (Schroeder, Würzner, cy moderated priming effects. Priming in the Heister, Geyken, & Kliegl, 2014) and 50 nonsuffixed pseudoword condition was only sig- pseudoword targets were created by changing nificant for adults with higher language profi- one letter from a real word that was not in the ciency (+1SD), z=1.74, p<.05, but not for adults target word set. Word and nonword targets were 133 RT Prime Type Adults Children Stimulus Example All participants Suffixed Word 593 (12) 1024 (36) kleidchen - KLEID Suffixed Nonword 597 (12) 1051 (38) kleidtum - KLEID Nonsuffixed Nonword 614 (13) 1045 (38) kleidekt - KLEID Unrelated 629 (14) 1087 (41) träumerei - KLEID Higher Language Proficiency (+1SD) Suffixed Word 588 (12) 900 (28) kleidchen - KLEID Suffixed Nonword 583 (12) 924 (30) kleidtum - KLEID Nonsuffixed Nonword 602 (12) 903 (28) kleidekt - KLEID Unrelated 620 (13) 974 (33) träumerei - KLEID Lower Language Proficiency (-1SD) Suffixed Word 599 (12) 1189 (48) kleidchen - KLEID Suffixed Nonword 613 (13) 1218 (51) kleidtum - KLEID Nonsuffixed Nonword 626 (14) 1239 (52) kleidekt - KLEID Unrelated 638 (14) 1229 (51) träumerei - KLEID Table 1. Response times (in ms) for children and adults, averaged across items for each participant. Standard errors are presented in parentheses. with lower language proficiency (-1SD), z=1.16, Moreover, the pattern of priming generalizes to p=.25. beginning readers with higher language profi- For children, proficiency played an even ciency: they show priming similar to that of more pronounced role than for adults: higher higher proficient adults. For children with lower proficiency children (+1SD) showed the same language proficiency, the effects did not reach pattern as higher proficiency adults, namely significance, but were clearly most pronounced priming from all related condition, z=3.03, in the suffixed word condition. z=2.02, z=2.96, all p<.05. In contrast, in lower We argue that there is a developmental proficiency children (-1SD) priming in none of gradient in the use of morphological information the conditions reached significance, although during reading acquisition, driven by language there was a numerical advantage from suffixed proficiency. Beginning readers with low lan- word primes in the mean reaction times (40ms guage proficiency are only able to benefit from faster compared to the unrelated condition). morpho-semantic information, if at all. More advanced lexical knowledge allows readers to 3 Conclusion extract morpho-orthographic information. Fol- lowing Andrews and Davis (1999) and Grainger Our results confirm recent evidence for French and Ziegler (2011), we assume that this happens adults (Beyersmann et al., 2014), showing that through segmentation of the affix in lower profi- the extent to which morphological information is ciency adults, as indicated by the priming effects exploited depends on language proficiency also of both suffixed prime conditions. Crucially, in German. Adults in the present study showed higher proficiency adult and even child readers morphological priming effects from suffixed with sophisticated lexical knowledge are able to word primes (kleidchen-KLEID), suffixed additionally use segmentation of the embedded pseudoword primes (kleidtum-KLEID) and also stem, therefore showing facilitation also in the nonsuffixed pseudoword primes (kleidekt- nonsuffixed prime condition. Our results high- KLEID) relative to unrelated words (träumerei- light the importance of lexical knowledge as a KLEID). Priming from the nonsuffixed further determinant of the ability to exploit mor- pseudoword condition did not continue to be sig- nificant with decreasing language proficiency. 134 phological structure in the word recognition pro- orthographic segmentation in visual word recogni- cess, especially in children. tion. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 1090- 1098. Reference Schroeder, S., Würzner, K.-M., Heister, J., Geyken, Andrews, S., & Davis, M. H. (1999). Interactive acti- A., & Kliegl, R. (2014). childLex: A lexical data- vation accounts of morphological decomposition: base for German read by children. Manuscript Finding the trap in mousetrap? Brain and Lan- submitted for publication. Max Planck Institute for guage, 68, 355-361. Human Development, Berlin. Andrews, S., & Lo, S. (2013). Is morphological prim- Taft, M. (2003). Morphological representation as a ing stronger for transparent than opaque words? It correlation between from and meaning. In E. As- depends on individual differences in spelling and sink & D. Sandra (Eds.) Reading complex words vocabulary. Journal of Memory and Language, 68, (pp. 113-137). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: 279-296. Kluwer. Beyersmann, E., Casalis, S., Ziegler, J. C., & Grain- ger, J. (2014). Language proficiency and morpho- Weiß, R. H. (1998). Grundintelligenztest Skala 2 orthographic segmentation. Psychonomic Bulletin (CFT 20) mit Wortschatztest (WS) und Zahlenfol- Review. gentest (ZF). Göttingen: Hogrefe. Beyersmann, E., Castles, A., & Coltheart, M. (2012). Morphological processing during visual word recognition in developing readers: Evidence from masked priming. The Quarterly Journal of Exper- imental Psychology, 65, 1306-1326. Grainger, J., & Ziegler, J. C. (2011). A dual-route approach to orthographic processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 45, Lemhöfer, K., & Broersma, M. (2012). Introducing LexTALE: A quick and valid Lexical Test for Ad- vanced Learners of English. Behavior Research Methods, 44, 325-343. Longtin, C. M., & Meunier, F. (2015). Morphological decomposition in early visual word processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 53, 26-41. Moll, K., & Landerl, K. (2010). SLRT-II: Lese- und Rechtschreibtest: Weiterentwicklung der Salzbur- ger Lese- und Rechtschreibtests (SLRT): Manual. Bern: Verlag Hans Huber. Morris, J., Porter, J. H., Grainger, J., & Holcomb, P. J. (2011). Effects of lexical status and morphological complexity in masked priming: An ERP study. Language & Cognitive Processes, 26, 558-599. Quémart, P., Casalis, S., & Colé, P. (2011). The role of form and meaning in the processing of written morphology: A priming study in French develop- ing readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psy- chology, 109, 478-496. Rastle, K., & Davis, M. H. (2008). Morphological decomposition based on the analysis of orthogra- phy. Language & Cognitive Processes, 23, 942- 971. Rastle, K., Davis, M. H., & New, B. (2004). The broth in my brother’s brothel: Morpho- 135