=Paper=
{{Paper
|id=Vol-1419/paper0009
|storemode=property
|title=The Effects of Age of Acquisition and Semantic Congruency on Famous Person Category Verification
|pdfUrl=https://ceur-ws.org/Vol-1419/paper0009.pdf
|volume=Vol-1419
|dblpUrl=https://dblp.org/rec/conf/eapcogsci/Smith-SparkMV15
}}
==The Effects of Age of Acquisition and Semantic Congruency on Famous Person Category Verification==
The effects of age of acquisition and semantic congruency on famous person category verification James H. Smith-Spark (smithspj@lsbu.ac.uk) Department of Psychology, London South Bank University, 103 Borough Road, London, SE1 0AA, UK Viv Moore (v.moore@gold.ac.uk) Department of Psychology, Goldsmiths, University of London, New Cross, London, SE14 6NW, London, UK Tim Valentine (t.valentine@gold.ac.uk) Department of Psychology, Goldsmiths, University of London, New Cross, London, SE14 6NW, London, UK Abstract sought to examine whether AoA effects could be obtained on a semantic processing task requiring responses to the The age of acquisition (AoA) effect, a processing advantage for items learnt earlier in life, affects naming and making names of famous people. Mature adults were presented with familiarity decisions about famous people. However, its a category verification task in which they were required to influence on semantic processing tasks involving celebrity indicate whether the name of a famous person was stimuli is equivocal. In a category verification task designed associated with a particular area of fame. Some interaction to explore this issue further, mature adults were shown an between AoA and semantic congruency has been suggested area of fame, followed by a famous person’s name. They in the processing of words by Ghyselinck, Custers, and were asked to indicate whether the area of fame and the celebrity matched. Stimulus congruency and AoA were Brysbaert (2004). A natural consequence of the manipulated orthogonally, with familiarity and facial categorization task allowed congruency also to be explored distinctiveness being controlled. Faster and more accurate to determine whether further (and stronger) evidence for this responses were produced when the area of fame and the interaction would be found when processing famous names. celebrity matched. Faster and more accurate responses were Age of acquisition effects have been found on a number made to early-acquired celebrities but the interaction fell short of different types of semantic processing tasks in the lexical of significance but is consistent with that reported for lexical and object processing domains (e.g., Belke et al., 2005; processing. With adequate control of extraneous variables and an extended distance between stimulus groups, AoA would Brysbaert, van Wijnendaele, & De Deyne, 2000; Ghyselinck seem to have an influence on the semantic processing of et al., 2004; Johnston & Barry, 2005). These findings have famous people and interacts near significance with been used to support a hypothesis that proposes that the congruency. The results are considered in the light of multiple effects of AoA originate, at least partly, from a semantic loci theories of AoA. locus (e.g., Brysbaert et al., 2000). The semantic hypothesis argues that the greater the level Keywords: Age of acquisition; Semantic processing; of involvement of semantic representations in task Congruency effects; Name categorization performance, the greater the effects of AoA are likely to be (e.g. Brysbaert et al., 2000). Other loci are not ruled out by Introduction this account, but the semantic processing system is posited People are faster and more accurate when processing words to play a role in producing AoA effects. Brysbaert et al. and objects that they have learnt earlier in life than those argue that the order in which items are acquired plays a acquired later (e.g., Morrison & Ellis, 1995; Moore, Smith- defining role in the way the semantic system is organized, Spark, & Valentine, 2004). This phenomenon, known as the with the meanings of later-acquired concepts depending on age of acquisition (AoA) effect, has been reported across a those of earlier-acquired items. The semantic hub network range of different processing tasks (see e.g., Juhasz, 2005, model of Steyvers and Tenenbaum (2005) is often cited in for a review). Moreover, its influence has been shown to support of the semantic hypothesis (e.g., Ghyselinck et al., remain robust after controlling for other variables known to 2004). According to this model, it is the greater number of influence processing speed, most importantly word semantic connections to other nodes (or concepts) possessed frequency (e.g., Cortese & Khanna, 2007; Peréz, 2007; by early-acquired items in their representational network Turner, Valentine, & Ellis, 1998). Whilst AoA effects on that is responsible for AoA effects rather than the order in naming and familiarity decisions are also well documented which nodes are acquired per se. Thus, semantic effects are in the people processing domain (e.g., Moore & Valentine, argued to be superordinate to AoA effects under the 1998, 1999), their influence on semantic processing tasks semantic hub network account. The predictions of the remains both underexplored and equivocal (e.g., Lewis, Steyvers and Tenenbaum model should generalize from the 1999a; Moore, 1998, 2003). Given this, the present paper 84 processing of words to the processing of famous people (see within his model, Lewis argued that AoA was a significant Smith-Spark, Moore, & Valentine, 2012, 2013). Moreover, predictor of RT on a semantic processing task. the semantic hypothesis argues for AoA across different However, Moore et al. (1999) identified a number of processing domains (e.g., Brysbaert et al., 2000). potential confounds that may have been present in Lewis’ There are several lines of evidence against the semantic data (see Lewis, 1999b, for a response). Firstly, the measure hypothesis. Firstly, Izura and Ellis’ (2004) investigation of of AoA was an estimate by the experimenter of the number second language (L2) learning indicated that AoA effects in of instances that should have been in the participants’ L2 reflect the order in which words have been learnt in L2 memory (i.e., “familiarity”, Moore & Valentine, 1998; or rather than the age at which the corresponding words were “frequency of encounter”, Moore, 2003), and no subjective learnt in the first language (L1). It is difficult for the measures of AoA, familiarity, or facial distinctiveness were semantic hypothesis to explain this result, as semantic taken from the participants themselves. Such ratings have representations should be shared between L1 and L2. Izura typically been taken when investigating both frequency and Ellis argue that this finding indicates that AoA effects (e.g., Valentine & Moore, 1995) and AoA effects in the are not limited solely to the semantic level of representation. processing of famous people (e.g., Moore & Valentine, Secondly, Menenti and Burani (2007) compared 1998, 1999). In the lexical processing domain, it has been participants’ responses on a lexical decision to those on a argued that obtaining subjective ratings from participants is semantic categorization task. In contrast to what would be superior to obtaining frequency measures from word predicted by the semantic hypothesis, the magnitude of the corpora (e.g., Gernsbacher, 1984; although see Brysbaert & AoA effect was no greater on the semantic categorization Cortese, 2011, for a dissenting view). There is no reason to task than on the lexical decision task. Thirdly, data from the assume that it should be different in the famous name processing of famous names argue against the pre-eminence processing domain and this has been argued elsewhere (e.g., of semantic connectedness over AoA proposed by Steyvers Smith-Spark et al., 2012). The absence of subjective and Tenenbaum (2005). Smith-Spark et al. (2013) found a measures is compounded by a further assumption that actors strong main effect of AoA in the absence of a main effect of were best known for their one soap-opera role. However, a semantic variable (the amount of biographical information fame preceded the programme for some actors, whilst other known about a celebrity) on a famous name familiarity celebrities had left the programmes to appear in decision task. However, there was a role for the semantic contemporary top-rated British television series and plays. processing system in mediating the processing of late- Furthermore, the stimuli represented close semantic acquired celebrities. Knowing more about a celebrity led to associates, where response latencies could have been faster responses to late-acquired, but not early-acquired, affected by semantic or associative priming (Bruce & stimuli. Moreover, Smith-Spark et al. (2012, 2013) have Valentine, 1985). A raised level of semantic activation may argued that AoA effects on semantic processing may have occurred due to the large number of celebrities derived become more salient on people processing tasks when the from the same category (Sergent & Poncet, 1990). semantic processing system is involved to a greater extent. Reanalysis of the data by Moore (2003) suggested a more Typically, the investigation of AoA effects in the people parsimonious interpretation of the results. Classification processing domain has used celebrities as stimuli. A similar times were found to be significantly faster for pairs of soap processing advantage for early-acquired celebrities has been actors who were from the same soap family than pairs who found when participants are asked to name the faces of were not. Examination of the stimuli indicated that there celebrities (e.g., Moore & Valentine, 1998), read aloud their were more familial pairs of early-acquired celebrities. As a printed names (Moore & Valentine, 1999) and to make result, a greater level of semantic priming may have familiarity decisions about names or faces (e.g., Moore & occurred when responses were made to early-acquired Valentine, 1999; Smith-Spark, Moore, & Valentine, 2012, famous people and may have led to Lewis’ findings. 2013). However, the contribution of the semantic processing Moore (2003) did not obtain a processing advantage for system to the AoA effects reported in the people processing early-acquired famous people on a number of semantic domain is less clear. classification tasks, despite robust AoA effects being Lewis (1999a) proposed an instance-based model of AoA evident when the same celebrity stimuli were presented in effects to account for the influence of AoA on recognition naming and perceptual tasks (Moore & Valentine, 1998, and naming tasks. This model explains the categorization 1999). Of Moore’s experiments, six tasks revealed a non- speed of a stimulus as a function consisting of a negative significant processing advantage for early-acquired items power of the number of instances of a stimulus in memory and three revealed an advantage for late-acquired stimuli, of (i.e. its frequency of encounter) and the time period over which only one difference was statistically significant (and which the stimulus was encountered and a positive power of even this was not replicated in a subsequent experiment). the time since last exposure to it (i.e., its recency). Lewis’ Moore suggested that the lack of an early-acquired demonstration of a cumulative frequency effect was based advantage on these semantic tasks involving the faces or on a study in which the participants categorized faces as names of celebrities may have been due to only young those of actors appearing in one of two very well-known adults aged 18 to 25 years being tested. In her 2003 studies, British television soap operas. Whilst not explicitly tested an early-acquired celebrity was rated as having been 85 acquired between six to 12 years of age and a late-acquired items was twice the size of that for late-acquired stimuli, but celebrity was rated as having been acquired after 18 years of this congruency x AoA interaction fell short of statistical age. The two stimulus groups were, thus, separated by a significance (p = .10). Ghyselinck et al. argued that this period of only six years. Moore argued that individual and result suggested that the meanings of early-acquired words familial interests will influence the extent to which children were activated faster than those of late-acquired words. are exposed to certain celebrities (e.g., with, perhaps, a Consistent with previous research on people processing sporting, musical, or political bias). Such arbitrary tasks (e.g., Moore & Valentine, 1998, 1999; Smith-Spark et influences would not present the same stimulus selection al., 2012, 2013), it was predicted that an AoA effect would problem in object and lexical studies except with the most emerge after careful control of familiarity and facial technical and domain-specific of stimuli. In other words, distinctiveness. A congruency effect was also hypothesized, people’s language experiences within the same culture are in accordance with previous findings of semantic likely to be more similar than their interests and hobbies, congruency effects in different domains (words: e.g., De which may diverge considerably and, therefore, lessen the Houwer, 1998; faces: e.g., Barrett & Rugg, 1989). Faster chances of uncovering an AoA effect. RTs were expected on trials where there was a match Given the concerns relating to both Lewis (1999a) and between the area of fame and the subsequently presented Moore (2003), it has yet to be demonstrated conclusively celebrity. Whilst Ghyselinck et al.’s (2004) AoA x that AoA can influence semantic classifications on person congruency interaction fell short of statistical significance, a processing tasks. Therefore, the current experiment was run similar pattern of results was expected with famous names. in order to determine whether a semantic AoA effect on people processing could be found after removing the Method problems identified by Moore (2003; see also Lewis, 1999b). To this end, Moore’s (2003) category verification Participants task was used. Mature adult participants were requested to Twenty-four mature adults (14 female, 10 male; mean age = make Yes/No judgements as to whether there was a match 68 years, SD = 9) received a small honorarium for between an area of fame (such as politics or film) and a participating. All 24 reported that they had been UK subsequently presented celebrity (in the form of a residents for their entire lives. photograph of his or her face). Equal numbers of congruent trials (in which the celebrity matched the preceding area of Materials fame) and incongruent trials (in which the celebrity did not A PC running the E-Prime experiment generator software match the presented semantic category) were presented. package (Psychology Software Tools, Inc., Sharpsburg, PA) Two important alterations were made to Moore’s was used to administer the experiment. Responses were experimental design. Firstly, a greater number of stimuli made using a push-button response box. were used. Secondly, mature adults (aged 40+ years) were Ninety-six famous face stimuli were selected from Smith- recruited as participants in order to permit a greater Spark et al.’s (2006) database of famous names. The stimuli separation between early- and late-acquired items (resulting were manipulated in such a way as to provide subgroups in a gap of 30 years rather than six years). Stimuli were that were orthogonally different on measures of AoA but selected based on ratings taken from a large group of mature matched for familiarity and facial distinctiveness. Twenty- adults who did not take part in the experiment (Smith-Spark four stimuli were drawn from each of the four areas of fame et al., 2006). (comedy, film, politics, and music). A relationship between AoA and semantic congruency Of these 96 stimuli, twenty-four early-acquired and 24 was expected on the basis of previous research. De Houwer late-acquired celebrities were deployed in congruent trials, (1998) found that faster responses were elicited when a where there was a match between area of fame and the participant’s verbal response was congruent with the famous person. Another 48 celebrity stimuli were used in meaning of the stimulus and slower responses were incongruent trials. There were, likewise, 24 early-acquired produced when the response and the stimulus were and 24 late-acquired famous names making up the incongruent. Ghyselinck et al. (2004) adapted De Houwer’s incongruent trials. A one-way analysis of variance task to investigate how AoA influenced semantic conducted on the a priori AoA ratings taken from Smith- processing. Ghyselinck et al. matched stimuli for familiarity Spark et al. (2006) database indicated a significant and manipulated AoA. Half their participants were difference between early- and late-acquired celebrities instructed to say ‘living’ when presented with words in (F(3,92) = 117.04, p < .001). Post hoc Bonferroni lower case and to say “non-living” to words presented in comparisons indicated that the significant differences in upper case. The remaining participants were asked to do the AoA were found between both early-acquired stimulus reverse. Half the words presented to participants belonged to groupings and both incongruent stimulus groupings (all p < living things and half to non-living. Ghyselinck et al. found .001). No other differences were significant. The Smith- significant effects of congruency (both by participants and Spark et al. database was also used to match the stimulus by items) on RT and a significant effect of AoA by items. groupings for the number of times their names had been The magnitude of the congruency effect on early-acquired generated (without recourse to reference works; indicating 86 the extent to which the celebrities were to the fore of age 15 years, and then rising in 10 year increments to 10, participants’ thoughts), the number of syllables in their being a celebrity acquired before the age of 85). names, their subjective familiarity, and their facial distinctiveness (F ≤ 1.30, p > .05). Facial distinctiveness has Results been found to affect RTs even when names rather than faces Responses more than 2.5 SD from the overall mean RT of are used as stimuli (Moore, 1998). each participant were removed from the data set prior to the analyses being performed. A total of 54 trials out of 2303 Design were removed (2.34%). Findings can be generalized over both participants (F1) and Following the data trimming, two stimuli were left out of items (F2) by the use of multilevel modelling analysis (e.g., the analysis, one due to low accuracy of response (Tom Brysbaert, 2007). Separate multilevel modelling analyses Jones = 58%) and one (Rod Stewart) due to participant were performed on the reaction time (RT; ms) and accuracy ratings placing the stimulus in the late-acquired rather than (%) data with AoA (early-acquired vs. late-acquired) and the early-acquired grouping (mean AoA rating = 6.14). stimulus congruency (congruent vs. incongruent) were All remaining items were responded to with accuracies in entered as fixed factors, together with the AoA x stimulus excess of 70% correct. The analyses which follow were congruency interaction. Participant number and stimulus based on this reduced data set. number were entered into the analysis as random factors. Text was presented in reverse video Courier New font. Participant ratings The famous names appeared in 12-point and the semantic The participant ratings confirmed the validity of the a categories in 24-point font. priori allocation of congruent stimuli to the early- and late- acquired groupings. The early-acquired congruent items Procedure were rated as having been acquired significantly earlier than The participants gave their informed consent to take part in the late-acquired congruent stimuli, F(1, 45) = 131.59, MSE the experiment. They were told that on each trial they would = .496, p < .001, ηp2 = .745. The congruent stimulus groups be shown the name of one of four areas of fame (comedy, were well matched on ratings of familiarity, F(1, 45) < 1, film, music, and politics), followed by the name of a famous MSE = .364, p = .366, and facial distinctiveness, F(1, 45) < person. The participants were asked to indicate as quickly 1, MSE = .702, p = .913. and accurately as possible whether or not the famous name matched the preceding area of fame, by pressing the Reaction time appropriate key on a response box (labelled ‘Yes’ for Multilevel modelling analyses indicated that faster matching and ‘No’ for non-matching). At the start of each responses were made to early-acquired words (mean = trial, an orienting asterisk appeared on the monitor screen 1561ms, SD = 526) than late-acquired words (mean = for 700ms. The asterisk was replaced by a black screen and 1660ms, SD = 540). This effect of AoA on RT was found to the presentation of a 2000 Hz tone (250ms in duration). One be highly significant, F(1, 2053) = 18.03, p < .001. of the four areas of fame was then shown for 1500ms, Congruent stimuli (mean = 1528ms, SD = 491) were followed by the famous name presented centrally on the responded to more rapidly than incongruent stimuli (mean = screen. A Yes/No push-button response terminated the 1697ms, SD = 566). The effect of congruency was also display and initiated the next trial. In order to familiarize statistically highly significant, F(1, 2053) = 52.64, p < . 001. participants with the task demands, a practice session of 15 There was a trend towards a greater influence of trials preceded the experiment. congruency on early- than late-acquired stimuli (see Figure At the end of the task, the participants rated the congruent 1), but the AoA x stimulus congruency interaction fell short items1 for familiarity, distinctiveness, and AoA as follows: of statistical significance, F(1, 2053) = 2.91, p = .088. Familiarity: How often each celebrity had been encountered over time and across different media (from 1 = Accuracy completely unknown through to 7 = very familiar). Multilevel modelling analyses indicated that semantic Distinctiveness: How easy each famous person would be categorization decisions were more accurate to the names of to spot on a crowded railway platform based on facial 1 Participant ratings were not taken on the distractor items at the features alone (Valentine & Bruce, 1986). Ratings were time of testing. The ratings were limited to congruent items in made from 1, being a ‘typical’ face, hard to distinguish, to 7, order to retain the goodwill of participants (who would otherwise being a highly distinctive face, easy to pick out in a crowd. have had to rate 96 stimuli on each of the three dimensions). Data AoA: The participants rated when they first became aware collection was conducted some years ago, so it would not be of each celebrity on a 10-point scale (with a score of 1 possible to collect ratings even if the participants could be traced. indicating a famous person that the participant first became However, given that the participant ratings for the congruent items aware of before the age of five years, a score of 2 showed strong positive correlations with the a priori ratings representing a celebrity first encountered before 10 years of (familiarity, r(48) = .761, p < .001; distinctiveness, r(48) = .838, p < .001; AoA, r(48) = .965, p < .001), it is likely that a similar age, a score of 3 reflecting a famous person acquired before pattern would emerge with the incongruent items as they came from the same database. 87 decisions (e.g., Moore & Valentine, 1998). In previous studies (e.g., Moore, 2003), the range of AoA values over which stimuli could be selected was constrained by the relative youth of the participants. The use of a mature population in the present study allowed for a much wider separation between the early- and late-acquired AoA stimulus groupings. In combination with the selection of only the most familiar celebrities (based on scores from Smith-Spark et al., 2006, and validated by participant ratings) and a task drawing on greater levels of semantic processing (Smith-Spark et al., 2012, 2013), this has allowed semantic AoA effects on the processing of people’s names to be captured. Regardless of whether a participant has a particular subjective interest in a given domain of fame or individual celebrity, it is hard to escape the mention of highly famous people in the media. Less stringent control in the matching of stimuli and the use of younger adult participants may thus explain the previous null results on Figure 1: AoA x congruency interaction for RT. tasks involving the semantic processing of famous names (Moore, 2003). early-acquired celebrities (mean = .93, SD =.25) than to the Stimulus congruency and AoA would seem to interact at names of late-acquired famous people (mean = .89, SD around statistical significance across different processing =.31). The effect of AoA on accuracy was found to be very domains. This finding adds further weight to Ghyselinck et significant, F(1, 2251) = 9.69, p = .002. al.’s argument that there is greater semantic activation for Responses on congruent trials were also more accurate early-acquired stimuli. More generally, the results argue for (mean = .93, SD = .26) than those on incongruent trials multiple loci of AoA effects (in line with current AoA (mean = .89, SD = 0.31). Congruency also had a very theories; e.g., Brysbaert et al., 2000; Ellis & Lambon Ralph, significant influence on accuracy, F(1, 2251) = 8.95, p = 2000; Moore & Valentine, 1999; Moore, 2003). These .003. consider AoA to be a general property of learning which can There was no interaction between AoA and congruency, be found across processing tasks and domains. The current F(1, 2251) < 1, p = .944. findings extend the empirically reported effects of AoA on semantic processing from words and objects to people Discussion processing, suggesting that AoA influences semantic In contrast to previous studies of semantic processing processing across a range of domains. involving famous names (e.g., Moore, 2003), a significant effect of AoA was found, with the familiarity and Acknowledgments distinctiveness of the stimuli being well-matched on both a This research was funded by an Economic and Social priori and participant ratings. The participants were faster to Science Research Council grant (R000429624208) awarded semantically categorize early-acquired than late-acquired to Viv Moore and Tim Valentine. The authors would like to famous names. These findings are consistent with those of thank Andrew Ellis and Simon De Deyne for their helpful Lewis (1999a), who also found a significant effect of AoA comments on an earlier version of this work. on the semantic categorization of faces rather than names. A congruency effect was also found in the current experiment. The participants were significantly faster and References more accurate in responding to congruent items than they Barrett, S. E., & Rugg, M. D. (1989). Event-related were when a mismatch occurred between the semantic potentials and the semantic matching of faces. category and the famous name. Age of acquisition and Neuropsychologia, 27, 913-922. stimulus congruency were not found to interact Belke, E., Brysbaert, M., Meyer, A. S., & Ghyselinck, M. significantly, although there was a trend towards faster (2005). Age of acquisition effects in picture naming: responding to early-acquired than late-acquired congruent Evidence for a lexical-semantic competition hypothesis. items. This is consistent with the pattern of data reported by Cognition, 96, B45-54. Ghyselinck et al. (2004) on a lexical processing task. The Bruce, V., & Valentine, T. (1985). Identity priming in the findings thus extend their research on AoA and stimulus recognition of familiar faces. British Journal of congruency from lexical processing to people processing. Psychology, 76, 373-383. It would appear that AoA confers a similar advantage on Brysbaert, M. (2007). “The language-as-fixed-effect the semantic processing of the names of early-acquired fallacy”: Some simple SPSS solutions to a complex famous people as it does on perceptual Yes-No familiarity problem (Version 2.0). Royal Holloway, University of London. 88 Brysbaert, M., & Cortese, M. J. (2011). Do the effects of Some words to talk about words (pp. 67-94). New York: subjective frequency and age of acquisition survive better Nova Science Publications. word frequency norms? Quarterly Journal of Moore, V., Smith-Spark, J. H., & Valentine, T. (2004). The Experimental Psychology, 64, 545–559. effects of age of acquisition on object recognition. Brysbaert, M., & Ghyselinck, M. (2006). The effect of age European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 16, 417-439. of acquisition: Partly frequency related, partly frequency Moore, V., & Valentine, T. (1998). Naming faces: The independent. Visual Cognition, 13, 992–1011. effect of age of acquisition on speed and accuracy of Brysbaert, M., van Wijnendaele, I., & De Deyne, S. (2000). naming famous faces. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Age-of-acquisition effects in semantic processing tasks. Psychology, 51, 485-513. Acta Psychologica, 104, 215-226. Moore, V., & Valentine, T. (1999). The effects of age of Cortese, M. J., & Khanna, M. M. (2007). Age of acquisition acquisition in processing famous faces and names: predicts naming and lexical-decision performance above Exploring the locus and proposing a mechanism. In M. and beyond 22 other predictor variables: An analysis of Hahn & S. C. Stoness (Eds.), Proceedings of the twenty- 2,342 words. Quarterly Journal of Experimental first annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society Psychology, 60, 1072-1082. (pp. 416-421). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. De Houwer, J. (1998). The semantic Simon effect. Moore, V., Valentine, T., & Turner, J. (1999). Age-of- Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51A, 683- acquisition and cumulative frequency have independent 688. effects. Cognition, 72, 305-309. Ellis, A. W., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2000). Age of Morrison, C. M., & Ellis, A. W. (1995). The roles of word acquisition effects in adult lexical processing reflect loss frequency and age of acquisition in word naming and of plasticity in maturing systems: Insights from lexical decision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: connectionist networks. Journal of Experimental Learning, Memory & Cognition, 21, 116-133. Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 1103- Peréz, M. A. (2007). Age of acquisition persists as the main 1123. factor in picture naming when cumulative word frequency Gernsbacher, M. A. (1984). Resolving 20 years of and frequency trajectory are controlled. Quarterly Journal inconsistent interactions between lexical familiarity and of Experimental Psychology, 60, 32-42. orthography, concreteness, and polysemy. Journal of Sergent, J., & Poncet, M. (1990). From covert to overt Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 256–281. recognition in a prosopagnosic patient. Brain, 113, 989- Ghyselinck, M., Custers, R., & Brysbaert, M. (2004). The 1004. effect of age of acquisition in visual word processing: Smith-Spark, J. H., Moore, V., & Valentine, T. (2012). Further evidence for the semantic hypothesis. Journal of Long-term age of acquisition effects in famous name Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and processing. Acta Psychologica, 139, 202-211. Cognition, 30, 550-554. Smith-Spark, J. H., Moore, V., & Valentine, T. (2013). Izura, C., & Ellis, A. W. (2004). Age of acquisition effects Determinants of famous name processing speed: Age of in translation judgement tasks. Journal of Memory and acquisition versus semantic connectedness. Acta Language, 50, 165-181. Psychologica, 142, 230-237. Johnston, R. A., & Barry, C. (2005). Age of acquisition Smith-Spark, J. H., Moore, V., Valentine, T., & Sherman, S. effects in the semantic processing of pictures. Memory & M. (2006). Stimulus generation, ratings, phoneme counts, Cognition, 33, 905-912. and group classifications for 696 famous people by Juhasz, B. J. (2005). Age-of-acquisition effects in word and British adults over 40 years of age. Behavior Research picture identification. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 684- Methods, 38, 590-597. 712. Steyvers, M., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2005). The large-scale Lewis, M. B. (1999a). Age of acquisition in face structure of semantic networks: Statistical analyses and a categorisation: Is there an instance-based account? model of semantic growth. Cognitive Science, 29, 41-78. Cognition, 71, B23-B39. Turner, J. E., Valentine, T., & Ellis, A. W. (1998). Lewis, M. B. (1999b). Are age-of-acquisition effects Contrasting effects of age of acquisition and word cumulative-frequency effects in disguise? A reply to frequency on auditory and visual lexical decision. Moore, Valentine and Turner (1999). Cognition, 72, 311- Memory & Cognition, 26, 1282-1291. 316. Valentine, T., & Bruce, V. (1986). The effects of Menenti, L., & Burani, C. (2007). What causes the effect of distinctiveness in recognising and classifying object faces. age of acquisition in lexical processing? Quarterly Perception, 15, 525-535. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 652-660. Valentine, T., & Moore, V. (1995). Naming faces: The Moore, V. M. (1998). The effects of age of acquisition in effects of facial distinctiveness and surname frequency. processing people’s faces and names. Unpublished PhD Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48A, 879- thesis, University of Durham, UK. 894. Moore, V. (2003). An alternative account for the effects of age of acquisition. In P. Bonin (Ed.), Mental lexicon: 89