=Paper=
{{Paper
|id=Vol-1419/paper0058
|storemode=property
|title=Effects of Emotion and Age on Cognitive Control in a Stroop Task
|pdfUrl=https://ceur-ws.org/Vol-1419/paper0058.pdf
|volume=Vol-1419
|dblpUrl=https://dblp.org/rec/conf/eapcogsci/BergerD15
}}
==Effects of Emotion and Age on Cognitive Control in a Stroop Task==
Effects of Emotion and Age on Cognitive Control in a Stroop Task Natalie Berger (n.berger@bbk.ac.uk) Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London, Malet Street London, WC1E 7HX, UK Eddy J. Davelaar (e.davelaar@bbk.ac.uk) Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London, Malet Street London, WC1E 7HX, UK Abstract congruent and incongruent trials relative to ‘neutral’ trials Research indicates that cognitive control is affected by aging (non-words such as XXXX printed in different ink colors) is and by emotion. However, no studies have addressed whether thought to facilitate a shift to proactive control due to high performance in an emotional Stroop task varies in older expectancy of conflict (e.g., Entel, Tzelgov, Bereby-Meyer, relative to younger adults. We examined the effect of aging in & Shahar, 2014). In contrast, reducing the amount of a classic Stroop color-naming task (Experiment 1) and in an congruent and incongruent trials relative to neutral trials is emotional Stroop task using faces (Experiment 2). Results thought to facilitate a shift away from proactive to reactive suggest that aging is associated with changes in cognitive control due to low expectancy of conflict. Similarly, trial- control but that older adults benefit more than younger adults from positive information in an emotional Stroop paradigm. by-trial changes in control modes can be introduced in cuing paradigms (e.g., Goldfarb & Henik, 2013). Incongruent Keywords: cognitive control; emotion; aging; Stroop task trials that are cued are thought to induce shifts to proactive control in contrast to non-cued items. Studies using the Background and Motivation Stroop task to test cognitive control usually report longer The ability to exert cognitive control in the presence of task- RTs for congruent than neutral trials (reversed facilitation) irrelevant information is crucial for adaptive cognitive and relatively increased RTs for incongruent compared to functioning in everyday life and has been studied neutral trials (interference) when proactive control is low extensively in experimental research. A commonly used (e.g., Goldfarb & Henik, 2007). task to examine cognitive control is the classic Stroop color- Despite a large body of research examining cognitive naming paradigm (Stroop, 1935). In this task, color words control, studies investigating the effect of aging on different are printed in congruent or incongruent ink (e.g., “red” control modes are relatively sparse to date. In studies using printed in red vs green ink) and participants have to name the AX-CPT task, which requires participants to maintain the color of the ink while ignoring the color word. While goal-related information and to make target responses on congruent items require the same response for both word cued trials and non-target responses on all other trials, older reading and color naming, word reading interferes with the adults were found to show significantly impaired goal correct color-naming response for incongruent trials. Thus, maintenance relative to younger adults (e.g., Haarmann, incongruent trials are typically associated with slower, less Ashling, Davelaar, & Usher, 2005). These results are accurate responses than congruent trials (MacLeod, 1991). usually interpreted as evidence for a reduction in proactive Although the exact mechanisms that enable cognitive control among older adults. However, such results have not control are still debated, there is reason to believe that been replicated in other experiments (e.g., Paxton, Barch, cognitive control is not a unitary process. According to the Racine, & Braver, 2008, Exp. 2) suggesting that age-related dual mechanisms of control (DMC) theory (Braver, Gray, & changes in proactive control are still poorly understood. Burgess, 2007), cognitive control operates in two distinct Reactive control seems relatively preserved in aging control modes, ‘proactive control’ and ‘reactive control’. In (Braver, 2012), but brain-imaging data suggest that reactive their theory, Braver et al. conceptualize proactive control as control is associated with an increase in transient activation an actively sustained control mode, which is initiated before in older relative to younger adults (Paxton et al., 2008). This the occurrence of conflict. Thus, proactive control is thought pattern of results was interpreted as evidence that older to bias attention and action systems in a goal-directed way. adults might perform similar to younger adults by resorting In contrast, reactive control is activated by the occurrence of to compensatory mechanisms. However, more research is conflicting events and thus, is only mobilized when needed. needed to understand age-related changes in control modes. As both control modes are thought to be associated with As the results reported above have been found using the advantages and limitations (e.g., resource-demanding but AX-CPT paradigm, other tasks may help to shed light on fast mode of proactive control vs. parsimonious but slow age-related changes in cognitive control. mode of reactive control), it is suggested that both systems The Stroop task has also been adapted to investigate the are needed for successful cognitive performance. effects of emotion on cognitive control, often in the context The Stroop task has also been used to induce shifts in of anxiety or mood disorders. For instance, negative words control modes. For instance, increasing the amount of (e.g., ‘death’ printed in red) vs neutral words (e.g., ‘desk’ 370 printed in red) have been used to investigate interference information, older adults were even more accurate than caused by emotional content. Generally, participants were younger adults when updating positive information (Mikels, found to be slower in naming the color of emotional relative Larkin, Reuter-Lorenz, & Carstensen, 2005). to neutral words (Williams, Mathews, & MacLeod, 1996). Despite evidence of age-related changes in the effects of A common interpretation is that the affective nature of the emotion on attention, episodic memory and WM, to date, no emotional words interferes with color-naming by capturing study has examined cognitive control of emotional material attentional resources (Williams et al., 1996). Similar results in older as compared to younger adults. were observed for studies using pictures or faces as emotional stimuli (Kindt & Brosschot, 1997). Although Present studies some findings suggest that this effect is restricted to We report results of two behavioral studies investigating negative items (McKenna & Sharma, 1995), other studies proactive and reactive control in aging. The first experiment have shown that both positive and negative pictures focused on neutral material in the classic Stroop color- interfered stronger with color-naming than neutral pictures naming paradigm. In the second experiment, we extended as evidenced in longer RTs (Constantine, McNally, & the task to include emotional material by using facial Hornig, 2001). In a more recent study, Krug and Carter stimuli. Cognitive control was manipulated by varying the (2012) reported higher interference by irrelevant emotional amount and thus, expectancy of congruent and incongruent relative to irrelevant neutral information in a Stroop task. trials relative to neutral trials as in the study of Tzelgov, This effect was marginally more pronounced under low than Henik, and Berger (1992). High-expectancy (HE) blocks under high expectancy conditions and thus, when proactive were thought to be associated with increased proactive control was low. However, as in previous studies emotional control, whilst low-expectancy (LE) blocks were thought to information was task-irrelevant, it is less clear how task- be associated with reduced proactive control. relevant emotion would affect performance in a Stroop task. Based on previous research, we expected to find reduced As suggested by the dual-competition model (DCM; Pessoa, cognitive control in older adults in Experiment 1. In 2009), emotion can improve cognitive performance through Experiment 2, we expected that task-relevant emotion enhanced target processing. Thus, it is possible that in a would facilitate Stroop performance and reduce age-related Stroop paradigm, the processing of target emotional differences. It was an open question, whether task-relevant information can be more resilient against distraction. emotion would interact with expectancy of conflict. More importantly, no previous study has investigated how aging affects cognitive control in an emotional Stroop task. Experiment 1 It is important to consider age-related changes when investigating the interaction between emotion and cognitive Methods control, as aging is associated with preserved or even Subjects. Twenty younger (ages 20–40) and 20 older (ages improved emotional functioning (Charles & Carstensen, 60–85) participated in the experiment. Younger adults were 2010). Older adults seem to place greater importance on students at Birkbeck, University of London, and received emotional information compared to younger adults (Fung & either course credits or £7.50/hour for their participation. Carstensen, 2003). It was argued that an increased striving Older adults were high-functioning volunteers, who were for emotional well-being might underlie age-related changes recruited from the University of Third Age in London and in emotional functioning. For instance, socioemotional were paid £7.50/hour. Participants were community- selectivity theory (SST; Carstensen, 1993) suggests that dwelling, pre-screened for psychiatric disorders and a perceiving time as limited promotes a prioritization of history of neurological disorders and had normal or emotional well-being. Consequently, older adults are corrected-to-normal vision. Subjects were tested thought to focus more on emotional and particularly on individually. positive information, resulting in a ‘positivity bias’ in Stimuli. Three color words were used in this experiment: attention and episodic memory (for a review, see Reed & red, green, yellow. Congruent items were created by Carstensen, 2012). For instance, it was found that older printing each of the color names in their own color (e.g., adults showed gaze preferences towards positive and away ‘red’ printed in red ink). Incongruent items were created by from negative stimuli (e.g., Isaacowitz, Wadlinger, Goren, printing each of the color names in one of the two other & Wilson, 2006) and that they remembered positive colors (e.g., ‘red’ printed in green or yellow ink). Neutral information better than negative information (Charles, items were created by printing a string of ‘XXXX’ in the Mather, & Carstensen, 2003). three different colors. The stimuli were presented centrally Other studies from the domain of working memory (WM) on black background in 42-point Courier New font. have shown that older adults can benefit from task-relevant Procedure. After giving consent and completing a short emotional rather than neutral information in cognitive tasks. visual acuity test, participants were instructed to perform the For instance, Mammarella and colleagues (2013a; 2013b) computerized Stroop task. The task consisted of two blocks, found that older adults showed poorer performance than and the order of the blocks was counterbalanced across younger adults in an operation WM span task with neutral participants. In the HE block, 75% of the trials were either but not emotional words. In an n-back task with emotional congruent or incongruent (37.5%, respectively), while 25% 371 of the trials were neutral. In the LE block, 25% of the trials were either congruent or incongruent (12,5%, respectively) and 75% of the trials were neutral. Each block consisted of 192 randomly ordered items and the task was preceded by a practice block of 24 items with an equal amount of congruent, incongruent and neutral items. Participants were instructed to indicate the color of the ink as quickly and accurately as possible by pressing one of three color-labeled buttons. Button presses initiated the next trials and the assignment of color labels to buttons was counterbalanced across participants. Median RTs for correct responses were analyzed as the main dependent variable in this experiment. Results and Conclusions Older adults were generally slower than younger adults, F(1, 38) = 21.49, MSE = 15280, p < .001. There was also a main effect of congruency, F(2, 76) = 74.52, MSE = 10480, p < .001, with significantly longer RTs for incongruent than congruent or neutral trials. No significant difference was observed between neutral and congruent trials across both conditions (p = .880). This main effect was qualified by a significant congruency × age interaction, F(2, 76) = 8.83, MSE = 10480, p = .004, as older adults showed a more pronounced RT difference between incongruent and congruent or neutral trials than younger adults. Although there was no main effect of condition (p = Figure 1: RT facilitation and interference in two age .178), there was a significant condition × age interaction, groups under low-expectancy (LE) and high-expectancy F(1, 38) = 8.47, MSE = 5091, p = .006, as older adults were (HE) conditions. Error bars represent standard errors. significantly slower under LE conditions than under HE conditions. No such effect was observed for younger adults. Both younger and older adults showed marginally Experiment 2 significant reversed facilitation (RT-congruent vs RT- neutral) and significantly increased interference under LE to Methods HE conditions, as revealed by a significant condition × Subjects. Twenty-four younger (ages 20–40) and 24 older congruency interaction, F(2, 76) = 12.66, MSE = 1759, p < (ages 60–85) participated in the experiment. Both younger .001. This interaction was qualified by a significant and older adults were recruited from the same pool of condition × congruency × age interaction, F(2, 76) = 8.85, volunteers as in Experiment 1 and met the same criteria. MSE = 1759, p < .001. This interaction was driven by higher They were compensated in the same way and none of the Stroop interference (RT-incongruent vs RT-neutral) in older subjects, who participated in Experiment 1, were included in adults, t(19) = 7.01, p < .001, relative to younger adults, p < Experiment 2. Subjects were tested individually. .001, t(19) = 3.96, p = .001, in the LE condition (Figure 1). Stimuli. Stimuli consisted of 36 images of faces from the Age-related effects were present but less pronounced in the FACES database (Ebner, Riediger, & Lindenberger, 2010), HE condition, where again higher interference was shown a validated set of photographs of naturalistic faces of by older adults, t(19) = 6.39, p < .001, than by younger different ages in front view. Faces showed angry, neutral or adults, t(19) = 4.41, p < .001. happy expressions (12 items per emotion). Faces were This pattern of results suggests that similarly to young selected in a way that age group (young, middle-aged, older) adults, older adults exhibited proactive control under HE and sex (male, female) of the face models were balanced conditions, as evidenced by facilitation and reduced equally in each emotion category. The faces had been rated interference in both age groups. Notwithstanding, aging was in a preliminary evaluation study and were selected based associated with changes in cognitive control as evidenced on high agreement ratings between younger and older raters. by an age-related increase in interference. Moreover, age- Angry, neutral and happy faces varied significantly in effects were more pronounced under LE conditions und their valence and angry and happy faces were matched for thus, when proactive control was low. Incongruent trials arousal. Congruent items were created by printing matching caused disproportionately longer RTs relative to neutral emotion labels across the emotional faces (e.g., happy face trials in older than in younger adults. This indicates that with ‘happy’ label). Incongruent items were created by Stroop-interference is affected by aging and interacts with printing non-matching emotion labels across the faces (e.g., available levels of control. happy face with ‘angry’ label). Neutral items were created 372 by printing a string of ‘XXXX’ across the faces. Face pictures were turned to grey-scale, whilst labels were printed in red, 38-point Courier New font, and placed between eyes and mouths of the faces. To facilitate label reading, labels appeared 100 ms before the face. Procedure. After giving consent and completing a short visual acuity test, participants were instructed to perform the computerized emotional Stroop task. The task consisted of two blocks and the proportion of congruent, incongruent and neutral in the HE block and in the LE was identical as in Experiment 1. Each block consisted of 288 randomly ordered items and the task was preceded by a practice block of 24 items with an equal amount of congruent, incongruent and neutral items. Participants were instructed to indicate the emotion of the face irrespective of the label as quickly and accurately as possible by pressing one of three labeled buttons (‘A’ for angry, ‘N’ for neutral, and ‘H’ for happy). A button press initiated the next trial and the assignment of Figure 2: RTs for emotional stimuli in two age groups labels to buttons was counterbalanced across participants. across items and conditions. Error bars represent standard Median RTs for correct responses were analyzed as the errors. main dependent variable in this experiment. Results and Conclusions General Discussion Older adults were generally slower than younger adults, The aim of the present research was to investigate how F(1, 46) = 20.03, MSE = 472052, p < .001. A main effect of emotion and aging affect cognitive control in a Stroop task. congruency, F(2, 92) = 42.24, MSE = 21342, p < .001, was Data from Experiment 1 show that older adults can deploy observed as RTs were faster for congruent than neutral proactive control in the face of varying task demand. This items and faster for neutral than for incongruent items. Both was evidenced by facilitation and reduced interference in younger and older adults showed increased interference the HE relative to the LE block. However, cognitive control under LE relative to HE conditions, as evidenced by a was found to be affected by aging in both conditions: Older significant condition × congruency interaction, F(2, 92) = adults showed increased interference from incongruent trials 4.73, MSE = 6897, p = .019. Facilitation was unaffected by relative to younger adults. More pronounced age-related condition. No further effects including the factor condition changes were observed when proactive control was low, as were observed. Emotion significantly affected RTs, F(2, 92) interference in older relative to younger adults was more = 21.17, MSE = 48104, p < .001, as all participants were increased under LE compared to HE conditions. faster for happy than neutral or angry faces. No significant Data from Experiment 2 suggest that emotion affects difference was found between angry and neutral faces (p > performance in a Stroop task irrespective of expectancy .05). However, this main effect was qualified by a manipulations and thus, task demands. It was found that significant emotion × age interaction, F(2, 76) = 3.95, MSE both age groups were fastest when responding to happy = 48104, p = .04. This interaction was driven by a more faces rather than neutral or angry faces. However, this effect pronounced RT advantage for happy faces relative to neutral was more pronounced for older adults as they showed a and angry faces in older as compared to younger adults higher speed advantage for happy faces than younger adults. (Figure 2). This facilitating effect of task-relevant positive emotion was These findings suggest that emotion modulated RTs observed across congruent, incongruent and neutral items. irrespective of item congruency or expectancy of conflict. In Both experiments shed light on age-related changes in the presence of emotional material, interference was cognitive control in the presence of neutral and emotional increased under LE conditions relative to HE conditions in material. The findings add to the empirical evidence both younger and older adults, with no effects of conflict obtained in studies using the AX-CPT paradigm. Our results expectation observed for facilitation. Happy faces were are compatible with studies showing impairments in goal associated with fastest RTs compared with neutral or angry maintenance in older adults, which were interpreted as an faces in both age groups. However, it was found that this age-related reduction in proactive control (Haarmann et al., RT advantage for happy faces was more pronounced for 2005). Under HE conditions, older adults showed increased older than for younger adults. These results suggest that interference from incongruent trials relative to younger older adults benefit from positive information as conveyed adults. This might indicate that active maintenance of task by smiling faces to a greater extent than younger adults do. goals was reduced in older adults, resulting in higher conflict experienced for incongruent stimuli. Moreover, our results extend previous findings by showing that word- 373 reading interfered stronger with color naming in older than These changes to the original Stroop task might have in younger adults when proactive control was low: older contributed to the discrepancy in results between the two adults were disproportionately slower than younger adults experiments. Faces rather than words were used as they are when responding to incongruent relative to neutral items ecologically valid emotional stimuli, whereas words convey under LE conditions. Given the implication by the dual emotional meaning in a symbolic way. A comparison of mechanisms of control (DMC) theory that a shift away from stimulus types indeed showed stronger effects of valence for proactive control facilitates the use of reactive control pictorial stimuli than for words (Kensinger & Schacter, (Braver et al., 2007), our results suggest that reactive control 2006). Also, it was suggested that even with word stimuli, might also be affected by aging. Previous studies have emotional Stroop tasks differ from the Stroop color-naming shown that reactive control is largely preserved in older task for a number of reasons (Algom, Chajut, & Lev, 2004). adults (Braver, 2012), but there is also evidence that older For instance, it was argued that the Stroop effect cannot be adults’ reactive control is less efficient, as compensatory calculated in an emotional Stroop as no “true” consistency mechanisms might be recruited at neural levels (Paxton et exists for non-color words printed in different colors. Thus, al., 2008). Our results are in line with those pointing at less the inclusion of stimuli other than color words comes with efficient reactive control in older relative to younger adults. major methodological changes, independently of stimulus Finally, the finding of increased interference in older adults type. is consistent with findings of reduced inhibition of irrelevant To conclude, our studies extend previous research by information with aging (e.g., Hasher, Stoltzfus, Zacks, & showing that age-related changes were more pronounced in Rypma, 1991). a Stroop task when proactive control was low. Furthermore, The results from Experiment 2 suggest that emotion it was shown that performance in an emotional Stroop task affects Stroop performance irrespective of expectancy was affected by aging and by emotion. More specifically, across all trial types. Both age groups were fastest when older adults were disproportionately faster when responding responding to happy faces, but this RT advantage was more to happy rather than angry or neutral faces compared to pronounced in older than in younger adults. The speed-up in younger adults. Further research is needed to clarify whether responses for happy faces is in line with research showing this happy face advantage was driven by perceptual or that happy expressions are processed quicker and more conceptual features of the stimuli. By furthering our accurately relative to other facial expressions (Becker & understanding of the effects of emotion on cognitive control Srinivasan, 2014). However, a purely perceptual account of in aging, such work can help identifying situations in which our results would imply that older adults are faster or better older adults’ cognitive performance can be facilitated in recognizing happy faces than younger adults, which is not through emotion. supported by research (for a meta-analysis, see Ruffman, Henry, Livingstone, & Phillips, 2008). In contrast, a more Acknowledgments pronounced RT advantage for happy faces in older relative This work was supported by Birkbeck, University of to younger adults is more consistent with a conceptual view London. of emotional changes in aging and findings showing a ‘positivity bias’ in attention and memory in aging (Reed & Carstensen, 2012). It is possible that the emotion × age References interaction in Experiment 2 was driven by motivational Algom, D., Chajut, E., & Lev, S. (2004). A rational look at changes in aging, with older adults focusing more on the emotional Stroop phenomenon: A generic slowdown, positive information and thus, responding faster to happy not a Stroop effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: faces relative to younger adults. However, as both age General, 133, 323-338. doi: 10.1037/0096- groups generally showed an RT advantage for happy faces, 3445.133.3.323 it is likely that perceptual and conceptual features of happy Becker, D. V., & Srinivasan, N. (2014). The vividness of faces contributed to the observed results. Our stimuli do not the happy face. Current Directions in Psychological allow disentangling these effects and future studies using Science, 23, 189-194. doi: 10.1177/0963721414533702 different stimuli such as words or pictures are needed to Braver, T. S. (2012). The variable nature of cognitive investigate this question further. control: a dual mechanisms framework. Trends in It should be noted that Experiment 2 did not replicate the Cognitive Sciences, 16, 106-113. doi: pattern of results obtained in Experiment 1, particularly with 10.1016/j.tics.2011.12.010 regards to differences in LE and HE conditions. Although Braver, T. S., Gray, J. R., & Burgess, G. C. (2007). increased interference was observed under LE conditions Explaining the many varieties of working memory relative to HE conditions, no effect of condition was variation: Dual mechanisms of cognitive control. In A. R. observed for facilitation. Methodological differences in task A. Conway, C. Jarrold & M. J. Kane (Eds.), Variation in design may account for the different results observed. In working memory (pp. 76-106). New York, NY, US: Experiment 2, we used facial expressions with labels to Oxford University Press. create incongruent, congruent and neutral stimuli. A 100 ms Carstensen, L. L. (1993). Motivation for social contact label preview was also introduced before the face appeared. across the life span: A theory of socioemotional selectivity. In J. E. Jacobs (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on 374 Motivation: Developmental perspectives on motivation of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 644-648. doi: (Vol. 40, pp. 209-254). Lincoln, NE: University of 10.1037/0021-843X.106.4.644 Nebraska Press. Krug, M. K., & Carter, C. S. (2012). Proactive and reactive Charles, S. T., & Carstensen, L. L. (2010). Social and control during emotional interference and its relationship emotional aging. Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 383- to trait anxiety. Brain Research, 1481, 13-36. doi: 409. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100448 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.08.045 Charles, S. T., Mather, M., & Carstensen, L. L. (2003). MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Aging and emotional memory: The forgettable nature of Stroop effect: An integrative review. Psychological negative images for older adults. Journal of Experimental Bulletin, 109, 163-203. doi: 10.1037/0033- Psychology: General, 132, 310-324. doi: 10.1037/0096- 2909.109.2.163 3445.132.2.310 Mammarella, N., Borella, E., Carretti, B., Leonardi, G., & Constantine, R., McNally, R. J., & Hornig, C. D. (2001). Fairfield, B. (2013). Examining an emotion enhancement Snake fear and the pictorial emotional Stroop paradigm. effect in working memory: Evidence from age-related Cognitive Therapy and Research, 25(6), 757-764. doi: differences. Neuropsychological rehabilitation, 23, 416- 10.1023/A:1012923507617 428. doi: 10.1080/09602011.2013.775065 Ebner, N. C., Riediger, M., & Lindenberger, U. (2010). Mammarella, N., Fairfield, B., Di Domenico, A., Borella, FACES—A database of facial expressions in young, E., & Carretti, B. (2013). Is working memory affective in middle-aged, and older women and men: Development dementia of Alzheimer’s type? Neuroscience Discovery, and validation. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 351-362. 70, 1-4. doi: 10.7243/2052-6946-1-4 doi: 10.3758/BRM.42.1.351 McKenna, F. P., & Sharma, D. (1995). Intrusive cognitions: Entel, O., Tzelgov, J., Bereby-Meyer, Y., & Shahar, N. An investigation of the emotional Stroop task. Journal of (2014). Exploring relations between task conflict and Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and informational conflict in the Stroop task. Psychological Cognition, 21, 1595-1607. doi: 10.1037/0278- Research, 1-15. doi: 10.1007/s00426-014-0630-0 7393.21.6.1595 Fung, H. H., & Carstensen, L. L. (2003). Sending Mikels, J. A., Larkin, G. R., Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., & memorable messages to the old: Age differences in Carstensen, L. L. (2005). Divergent trajectories in the preferences and memory for advertisements. Journal of aging mind: Changes in working memory for affective Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 163-178. doi: versus visual information with age. Psychology and 10.1037/0022-3514.85.1.163 Aging, 20, 542-553. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.20.4.542 Goldfarb, L., & Henik, A. (2007). Evidence for task conflict Paxton, J. L., Barch, D. M., Racine, C. A., & Braver, T. S. in the Stroop effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: (2008). Cognitive control, goal maintenance, and Human Perception and Performance, 33, 1170-1176. doi: prefrontal function in healthy aging. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 10.1037/0096-1523.33.5.1170 1010-1028. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm135 Goldfarb, L., & Henik, A. (2013). The effect of a preceding Pessoa, L. (2009). How do emotion and motivation direct cue on the conflict solving mechanism. Experimental executive control? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(4), Psychology, 60, 347-353. doi: 10.1027/1618- 160-166. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.006 3169/a000205 Reed, A. E., & Carstensen, L. L. (2012). The theory behind Haarmann, H. J., Ashling, G. E., Davelaar, E. J., & Usher, the age-related positivity effect. Frontiers in Psychology. M. (2005). Age-related declines in context maintenance doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00339 and semantic short-term memory. The Quarterly Journal Ruffman, T., Henry, J. D., Livingstone, V., & Phillips, L. H. of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental (2008). A meta-analytic review of emotion recognition Psychology, 58A, 34-53. doi: and aging: Implications for neuropsychological models of 10.1080/02724980443000214 aging. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, 863- Hasher, L., Stoltzfus, E. R., Zacks, R. T., & Rypma, B. 881. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.01.001 (1991). Age and inhibition. Learning, Memory, and Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal Cognition, 17, 163-169. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.17.1.163 reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643- Isaacowitz, D. M., Wadlinger, H. A., Goren, D., & Wilson, 662. doi: 10.1037/h0054651 H. R. (2006). Selective preference in visual fixation away Tzelgov, J., Henik, A., & Berger, J. (1992). Controlling from negative images in old age? An eye-tracking study. Stroop effects by manipulating expectations for color Psychology and Aging, 21, 40-48. doi: 10.1037/0882- words. Memory & Cognition, 20, 727-735. doi: 7974.21.1.40 10.3758/BF03202722 Kensinger, E. A., & Schacter, D. L. (2006). Processing Williams, J. M. G., Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. M. emotional pictures and words: Effects of valence and (1996). The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. arousal. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, Psychological Bulletin, 120, 3-24. doi: 10.1037/0033- 6, 110-126. doi: 10.3758/CABN.6.2.110 2909.120.1.3 Kindt, M., & Brosschot, J. F. (1997). Phobia-related cognitive bias for pictorial and linguistic stimuli. Journal 375