=Paper=
{{Paper
|id=Vol-1419/paper0061
|storemode=property
|title=Neuropsychological Deficits in Adult HIV Infected Postnatally: a Pilot Study in Patients with Hemophilia
|pdfUrl=https://ceur-ws.org/Vol-1419/paper0061.pdf
|volume=Vol-1419
|dblpUrl=https://dblp.org/rec/conf/eapcogsci/RivaCP15
}}
==Neuropsychological Deficits in Adult HIV Infected Postnatally: a Pilot Study in Patients with Hemophilia==
Neuropsychological deficits in adult HIV infected postnatally: a pilot study in patients with hemophilia Silvia Riva (silvia.riva1@unimi.it) Department of Health Science, University of Milan, via A. di Rudinì 8 Milan, Italy Ilaria Cutica (ilaria.cutica@unimi.it) Department of Health Science, University of Milan, via A. di Rudinì 8 Milan, Italy Gabriella Pravettoni (gabriella.pravettoni@unimi.it) Department of Health Science, University of Milan, via A. di Rudinì 8 Milan, Italy European Institute of Oncology (IEO), Milan, Italy Abstract Clifford, Franklin, Woods et al., 2010). Also, the longitudinal observation of these patients has given Despite advances in the management of HIV infection with the introduction of combination antiretroviral therapy contrasting results: some studies have not shown any (cART), it is well known that HIV can directly infect the decline in neurocognitive functions (Grassi Clerici, Perin, central nervous system (CNS) and, as a result Zocchetti et al., 1995; Selnes, Miller, McArthur, Gordon, et neuropsychological impairments can be manifested. al., 1990) while others have (Applebaum, Otto, Richardson, However, in literature there are contrasting results on which & Safren, 2010; Ayuso-Mateos, Pereda, Del Barrio, cognitive functions are mainly affected, especially when Echevarria et al., 2000; Woods Iudicello, Moran, Carey et different HIV seropositive populations are considered. In this al., 2008;). study, we seek to determine whether seropositivity is In addition, some authors have argued that such associated with a poor neuropsychological performance in difficulties might be more related to the presence of patients infected postnatally, namely haemophilic patients. The results suggest that HIV infection is associated with important covariates, such as CD4 nadir count (Ellis, deficits in attention, short term spatial memory, phonemic Badiee, Vaida, Letendre et al., 2011; Heaton, Franklin, fluency, abstraction and visual recognition. Such results have Ellis, McCutchan et al, 2011), the time of infection important implications for day-to-day functioning, as the level (Ettenhofer, Hinkin, Castellon, Durvasula et al., 2009), drug of impairment detected may cause difficulties in completing abuse (Shimizu, Chow, Valcour, Masaki et al., 2011), common everyday tasks. cranial traumas, and several psychological alterations, rather than to the direct action of HIV virus. Keywords: HIV seropositivity; neuropsychological In our view, it is important to notice that impairments; haemophilia. neuropsychological impairments have been mainly studied in two HIV seropositive (henceforth, HIV+) populations: Introduction - patients infected vertically or perinatally (e.g. through Although neuropsychological profiles vary amongst breast milk), a condition that presents adjunctive cognitive HIV+ individuals, several estimates indicate that as many as disorders linked to neuro-developmental growth changes 50% of HIV+ individuals display some degree of (Rondanelli, Caselli, Arico, Maccabruni et al., 2002; Van neuropsychological impairment when impairment is derived Rossum, Gaakeer, Verweel, Hartwig et al., 2003) and from comparisons with normative performance standards neurological changes (Antinori, Arendt, Becker, Cherner et (e.g., Dawes, Suarez, Casey, Cherner et al., 2008). A recent al, 2007); meta-analysis revealed that the most severe forms of HIV - patients who contracted the infection postnatally or in associated neuropsychological impairments have decreased adulthood, often comprises participants who presented other since the widespread use of combination antiretroviral types of confounding factors, such as cocaine and opiates therapy (cART): Al-Khindi and colleagues (2011) found use, alcohol abuse (Buttner, 2011; Byrd, Robinson-Papp, less attentional, motor, and executive skill impairments in Rivera Mindt, Mintz et al., 2013;Lundqvist, 2010), and HIV+ individuals treated with cART. Notwithstanding, different risk behaviours (De Ronchi, Faranca, Berardi, several studies revealed that impairments in learning (e.g., Scudellari et al., 2002). Carey, Woods, Rippeth, Heaton et al., 2006; Maki, Cohen, By contrast, a population of HIV patients generally Weber, Little et al., 2009), verbal memory (Seider, Assawin infected postnatally at an older age, and that usually do not Gongvatana, Devlin et al., 2014) and prospective memory present the drug-user populations confounding factors, (e.g., Doyle, Loft, Morgan, Weber et al., 2013; Martin, consists of patients medically induced to HIV infection Nixon, Pitrak, Weddington et al., 2007) still occur even in through blood transfusions (Ettenhofer, Hinkin, Castellon, patients treated with cART (e.g., Grant et al., 2014; Heaton, Durvasula, et al., 2009). Hemophiliacs treated with factor 383 infusions before 1985 have been at risk of acquiring HIV cytomegalovirus infection, multiple sclerosis, stroke, (Brookmeyer & Goedert, 1989): several studies have shown seizures/epilepsy, drug use. that about 60–80% of patients with hemophilia, exposed to infected blood concentrates, contracted the HIV virus (e.g., Tests were also administered to a control group consisting Goedert, 1995). of fifteen adults (mean age 49,3+6.84), comparable for age The interest in neurocognitive dysfunctions in hemophilia and education (Mann–Whitney test: z=-1.41, p=.16; z=-.35, HIV+ patients is quite innovative in the literature. Findings p=.77, respectively). Participants’ educational and are often inconsistent and difficult to summarize (for a professional characteristics are presented in Table 1. review, see Riva, Cutica, Pravettoni, 2014); most of the early research findings are based on the Hemophilia Growth Table 1. Patients’ and controls’ educational and and Development Study (HGDS; Hilgartner, Donfield, employment characteristics Willoughby, Contant et al., 1993), a multicenter study of the HIV+ Controls long-term effects of HIV infection on growth and haemophiliacs (n = 15) neurodevelopment in HIV+ hemophilia children and (n = 15) adolescent, that found that such patients did not differ Primary 1 - significantly from HIV- hemophilic controls on a variety of school neuropsychological tests. However, as indicated in follow- Secondary 8 9 up studies of HGDS patients (Iudicello, Woods, Weber, Educational school Dawson et al., 2008; Loveland, Stehbens, Mahoney, Sirois level High 5 4 et al., 2000; Watkins Cool, Usner, Stehbens, et al. 2000) School there was a significant decline in neurocognitive functions University 1 2 such as memory, attention and language over 5 years, Currently occupied 9 11 directly related to a decline in immune functioning and to socio-educational covariates such as school absenteeism and Material a poorer academic achievement that frequently marked these young patients. Participants completed six neuropsychological tests from Although there are some data available in the context of the Italian Brief Neuropsychological Examination battery paediatric populations, studies are totally underrepresented (ENB: Mondini, Mapelli, Vestri, Arcara, et al., 2003) to in the adult population with mixed and confusing results investigate: (e.g., Riedel, Helmstaedter, Bülau, Durwen et al., 1992). - visual attention and cognitive processing speed (Trail Some studies found cognitive impairment in attention, Making Test A, in which participants have to connect a motor skills, and visual performance in HIV+ haemophiliacs series of 25 number in ascending order as quickly as to be related to the decrement of immunological possible: Tombaugh, 2004); functioning, especially when the CD4+ cell count was lower - visual attention and executive functioning (Trail Making than 200/mm3 (e.g., Blanchette, Smith, King, Fernandes- Test B, in which participants have to alternate between Penney et al., 2002). No clear data on cognitive impairment numbers and letters (1, A, 2, B, etc.) connecting them in of HIV+ haemophiliacs with CD4+ cell count higher than sequential order as quickly as possible:Tombaugh, 2004); 200 are available. - digit span memory (Letter-Number Sequencing from the The present study aimed to assess the presence and the WAIS-III, which measures the number storage capacity of extent of neuropsychological impairments in such a the working memory: Weschler, 1995); patients’ group. - phonemic fluency (Verbal Fluency test, in which participants have to say as many words beginning with a certain letter as possible in 60 seconds: Lezak, 1995), Method - abstraction (Italian Test of abstraction from the ENB, in Participants which, given three words, the participant have to tell what Fifteen HIV+ male haemophiliacs (mean age 45+8.4) the three concepts have in common: Mondini, et al., 2003). were administered neuropsychological tests. Patients were - visual recognition (Rey Tangled Lines Task, in which recruited through their treating physicians via the participants have to recognize the greatest possible number haemophilia and thrombosis outpatient clinics in three of figures within a table in which the figures are drawn Italian centres. The inclusion criteria were as follow: tangled: Rey, 1964). diagnosis of haemophilia, diagnosis of HIV with CD4+ counts consistently > 200cells/mm3, treatment with cART, Procedure age > 18. Exclusion criteria were diagnosis of AIDS, serious Patients were met immediately after a routine medical mental illness with a certificated diagnosis (e.g., major appointment, in which –among other things- overall clinical depression, anxiety, bipolar disorder) or known central status, including the presence of concomitant diseases, was nervous system pathology, including progressive multifocal evaluated. After signing the data protection form, they were leukoencephalopathy, brain cancer, neurosyphilis, active presented with the neuropsychological tests; each of them 384 dealt with the tests individually in a quiet room. According with 13 to 18 years as “high educational level”. Nine to each test requirements, the answers were recorded either patients and 9 controls fell into the low educational level, by the patient or by the experimenter. Patients completed and 6 patients and 6 controls into the high educational level. the tests in about twenty minutes. Controls were tested Low school level patients performed worse than the high individually in a quiet room; they completed the test in education group only in Trial Making B Test (Mann- about twenty minutes. Whitney Test: z=-2.125, p=.036). We then divided patients according to their occupational Results status: employed individuals on one side (n=9) versus unemployed or with disability pension individuals (n=6). As only three test out of six have the corrected score that We found that the unemployed/disability pension group has allows to correct raw scores for age and educational level, a worse performance than the employed group only in Trial we decided to analyse the raw data for all the tests, also Making A Test (Mann-Whitney Test: z=-2.239; p=.026). considered that age and education do not differs in the two As these results were inconsistent with most of the groups. existing literature, in addition we performed an ANCOVA Between groups comparisons. Table 2 shows the mean analysis, controlling for the effect of age, education and raw data for each neuropsychological test, for patients and working status. The analysis controlled for such covariates controls. revealed that patients performed worse than controls on Attention with regard to the Trail Making A Test (F=8.470, Table 2. Mean raw scores patients and controls p=.005), and Trail Making Test B (F=5.811, (standard deviation in parentheses) p=.022). However, the predicted main effect of education was not significant (F=4.89, p = .066, ηp2 = .004), neither Patients Controls was the predicted main effect of working status (F=.595, p = Tests (n =15)) (n=15) .455, ηp2= .004), and nor the main effect of age (F= 1.226, p = .292, ηp2 = .004). Although the introduction of Trial Making 60.93seconds* 32.27 seconds covariates reduced the distance between the two groups of Test A (24.52) (8.54) participants, they did not eliminate the effect of HIV Trial Making 173.53seconds 92.73 seconds infection on neuropsychological impairment completely. Test B (63.39) (9.59) Therefore the presence of these covariates (age, educational Digit Span 3.93 items 5.87 items level and working status) did not explain group differences memory per se. (1.71) (.74) Phonemic 27.53 words 34.14 words Discussion fluency (6.20) (5.06) Although neuropsychological impairments are well Test of 4.40 abstr. 5.73 abstr. described in some specific HIV populations, very few abstraction (1.29) (.46) studies investigated the cohort of adults seropositive patients with haemophilia, especially in the cART era. Rey Tangled 25.19 figures 4.67 figures Furthermore, the few existing studies on children and Lines Task (1.20) (.62) * adolescents, and on adults before the widespread use of In Trial Making Test A and B, a longer time corresponds to a worst cARTs, often obtain inconsistent results. performance Our pilot study, conducted on a small but well-controlled sample of HIV + hemophiliacs, reveals that such patients Results show that patients’ performance in each show signals of neuropsychological impairments when neuropsychological test is worse than the corresponding compared with the controls’ performances. performance by healthy controls. More in detail, we found In particular, considering the whole group of HIV+ that patients performed significant worse than controls in patients, we found an HIV detrimental effect on tests Trail Making A (T-test: t= 3.53, p=.001), in Trail Making B requiring attention and rapid information processing (Trail (T-test: t= 4.88, p<.0001), in Digit Span (T-test: t=-4.02, Making Test A, and Trail Making B for patients with low p<.0001), in Phonemic Fluency (T-test: t= -3.19, p=.003), educational level), consistently with the classic and in Rey Tangled Test (T-test: t=-5..836, p<.0001). conceptualization of HIV as a subcortical disease targeting frontal-striatal circuits supporting these abilities (e.g., Within-group comparisons. As the HIV literature reveals a Baldewicz, Leserman, Silva, Petitto et al., 2004; Reger, correlation between cognitive disorders and patients’ socio- Welsh, Razani, Martin et al., 2002). demographic factors such as education and job status, we Patient’s results on these two attention tasks, on the visual performed some comparisons to figure out whether the same recognition task (Rey Tangled Lines), and on phonemic effect holds for our participants. We thus divided both fluency are also consistent with one of the two existing groups into two into sub-groups according to the study on adult seropositive hemophiliacs, by Riedel and educational level. We considered participants with 5 to 8 colleagues (1992), who found that patients are impaired in education years as “low educational level”, and participants 385 visual attention, visuoperceptual speed, and verbal memory haemophilia and HIV), as well as holding down a full-time and fluency. However, they also found that patients’ job. Continued research into the mechanisms related to HIV impairment was a linear relationship with the decreasing of seropositivity and neurocognitive dysfunction may provide immune functioning, whereas we found deficits also in targets for meaningful interventions. patients with high immune functioning (as all our patients are). Our patients’ impairment on Digit span memory is in line with Riedel’s work, and with several previous findings Acknowledgments showing that deficits in memory, and in particular in short The first author was supported from Novo Nordisk grant term memory, are among the strongest cognitive complain “Changing possibilities in Haemophilia”. The authors wish in HIV (non-haemophilic) patients (e.g., Doyle Suarez, to thank prof. Mannuccio Mannucci for his support, and the Casey, Cherner et al., 2013; Maki, Cohen, Weber, Little et huand the AICE (Italian Association of Haemophilia al., 2009; Seider et al., 2014). Also, our data are consistent Centres) for allowing to conduct this study in three with results from the other existing study on adults Haemophilia centres. seropositive haemophiliacs, a case-study of four patients (Turnbull, Saling, Kaplan-Solms, Cohn, et al, 1991), according to which patients are impaired in visual memory and spatial perception. References The HIV literature reveals a correlation between cognitive Al-Khindi, T., Zakzanis, K.K., & van Gorp, W.G. (2011). disorders and socio-demographic factors (job status and Does antiretroviral therapy improve HIV-associated education); however, we did not find such interactions. cognitive impairment? A quantitative review of the Further studies will be needed to understand these results. literature. Journal of the International Taken together, our results reveal that also HIV+ Neuropsychological Society, 17: 956–969. hemophiliacs are impaired in several cognitive functions, as Antinori, A., Arendt, G., Becker, J.T., Cherner, M., Clifford, it happens to HIV+ patients infected vertically, as well as to D.B., et al. (2007). Updated research nosology for HIV- patients infected in adulthood. This result suggests that the associated neurocognitive disorders. Neurology, 69: cognitive impairments might be related predominantly to 1789–1799. the direct effect of the HIV virus, and that they are relatively Ayuso-Mateos, J.L., Pereda, M., Del Barrio, A., Echevarria, independent from the age of infection and from confounding S., Farinas, M.C., and Garcia Palomo, D. (2000). Slowed factors (such as drug abuse). However, the study is limited reaction time in HIV-1 seropositive intravenous drug by its design as a pilot study, and it suffers from users without AIDS. European Neurology, 44, 72-8. methodological challenges, such as potential instability of Applebaum, A.J., Otto, M.W., Richardson, M.A., and the data related to the small sample size. Future studies, Safren, S.A. (2010) Contributors to neuropsychological with larger samples, should be conducted to replicate these impairment in HIV-infected and HIV-uninfected opiate- findings and test for potential interaction effects among dependent patients. Journal of Clinical Experimental variables of interest. Neuropsychology, 32, 579–589 Furthermore, future studies are needed to compare these Baldewicz, T.T., Leserman, J., Silva, SG, Petitto, J., results with those obtained by a group of seronegative Golden, R.N., Perkins, D.O., & Evans, D.L. (2004). hemophiliacs, to investigate to what extent the combination Changes in neuropsycho-logical functioning with of both diseases could affect the results obtained. progression of HIV-1 infection: Results of an 8-year Another limitation of this study is the lack of data on MRI longitudinal investigation. AIDS and Behavior, 8:345– abnormalities and other neurological indicators which made 355 it impossible to link the neuropsychological assessment Blanchette, N., Smith, M.L., King, S., Fernandes-Penney, results with some neurobiological correlates or other A., & Read, S. (2002). Cognitive development in school- medical observations. age children with vertically transmitted HIV infection. On the other hand, the current study is strengthened by Developmental Neuropsychology, 21:223-241. selective inclusion criteria (such as the level of CD4 > 200, Brookmeyer, R., & Goedert, J.J. (1989). Censoringinan and the absence of a psychiatric disorder such as a diagnosis epidemic with an application to hemophilia-associated of depression/anxiety), that allows the evaluation of a AIDS. Biometrics 45, 325–335.doi:10.2307/ 2532057 precise and representative cohort. It considerably enriches Buttner, A. (2011). Review: The neuropathology of drug the currently scarce literature on European samples of HIV+ abuse. Neuropathology and Applied Neurobiology, haemophiliac adult patients in the cART era. 37:118–134. The level of neuropsychological impairment we detected Byrd, D.A., Robinson-Papp J., Rivera Mindt, R., Mintz, L., have relevant implications for day-to-day patients’ Elliott, K., Lighty, Q., & Morgello S. (2013). Isolating functioning: for instance they may cause difficulties in Cognitive and Neurologic HIV Effects in Substance- completing common everyday tasks, such as maintaining Dependent, Confounded Cohorts: A Pilot Study. Journal adherence to complex medication regimens (both for of the International Neuropsychology Society, 19: 463- 673. 386 Carey, C.L., Woods, S.L., Rippeth, J.D., Heaton, R.K., potent antiretroviral therapy: CHARTER Study. Grant I, & The Hiv Neurobehavioral Research Center Neurology, 75: 2087–2096. (HNRC) Group. (2006). Prospective memory in HIV-1 Heaton, R.K., Franklin, D.R., Ellis, R.J., McCutchan, J.A., infection. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Letendre, S.L., LeBlanc, S., Corkran, S.H., et al. (2011). Neuropsychology, 28: 536–548. HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders before and Dawes, S., Suarez, P., Casey, C.Y., Cherner, M., Marcotte, during the era of combination antiretroviral therapy: T.D., Letendre, S., Grant, I., et al. (2008). Variable Differences in rates, nature, and predictors. Journal of patterns of neuropsychological performance in HIV-1 NeuroVirology, 17: 3–16. infection. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Heaton, R.K., Marcotte, T,D,, Mindt, M.R., Sadek, J., Neuropsychology, 30: 613–626. Moore, D.J., Bentley, H., McCutchan, J.A., et al. (2004). De Ronchi, D., Faranca, I., Berardi, D., Scudellari, P., The impact of HIV-associated neuropsychological Borderi, M., Manfredi, R., & Fratiglioni, L., (2002). impairment on everyday functioning. Journal of the Risk Factors for cognitive impairment in HIV-1-Infected International Neuropsychology Society, 10:317-331. persons with different risk behaviours. Archives of Hilgartner, M. W., Donfield, S. M., Willoughby, A., Neurology, 59: 812-818. Contant, C., Evatt, B., Gomperts, E., Hoots, K., et al. Doyle, K.L., Loft, S., Morgan, E.E., Weber, E., Cushman, (1993). Haemophilia Growth and Development Study : C., Johnston, E., Grant, I., et al., (2013). Prospective Design, methods, and entry data. American journal of memory in HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders Pediatric Hematology/Oncology, 15: 208-218. (HAND): The neuropsychological dynamics of time Iudicello JE, Woods SP, Weber E, Dawson MS, Scott JC, & monitoring. Journal of Clinical and Experimental The HIV Neurobehavioral Research Center (HNRC) Neuropsychology, 35: 359-372. Group (2008). Cognitive mechanisms of switching in Ellis, R.J., Badiee, J., Vaida, F., Letendre, S., Heaton, R.K., HIV-associated category fluency deficits. Journal of Clifford, D., Collier, C.A., et al. (2011). CD4 nadir is a Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 30: 797– predictor of HIV neurocognitive impairment in the era 804. of combination antiretroviral therapy. AIDS 25: 1747– Lezak, M.D. (1995). Neuropsychological assessment. 1751. Oxford University Press, Oxford Ettenhofer, M.L., Hinkin, C.H., Castellon, S.A., Durvasula, Loveland, K. A., Stehbens, J. A., Mahoney, E. M., Sirois, P. R., Ullman, J., Lam, M., Myerset, H., et al. (2009). A., Nichols, S., Bordeaux, J. D., Watkins, J. M., et al. Aging, neurocognition, and medication adherence in (2000). Declining immune function in children and HIV infection. American Journal of Geriatric adolescents with Haemophilia and HIV infection: Psychiatry, 17: 281–290. Effects on neuropsychological performance. Journal of Giesbrecht, C.J., Thornton, A.E., Hall-Patch, C., Maan, E.J, Pediatric Psychology, 25: 309-322. Cote, H.C.F., Money, D.M., & Pick, X.N.(2014). Select Lundqvist, T. (2010). Imaging cognitive deficits in drug Neurocognitive Impairment in HIV-Infected Women: abuse. Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences, Associations with HIV Viral Load, Hepatitis C Virus, 3:247–275. and Depression, but Not Leukocyte Telomere Length. Maki, P.M., Cohen, M.H., Weber, K., Little, D.M., Fornelli, PLoS ONE 9(3): e89556. D., Rubin, L.H., Perschler, P., et al. (2009) Impairments Grant, I., Franklin, D. R., Deutsch, R., Woods, S. P., Vaida, in memory and hippocampal function in HIV-positive vs F., Ellis, R. J., ... & Heaton, R. K. (2014). Asymptomatic HIV negative women. Neurology 72: 1661–1668. HIV-associated neurocognitive impairment increases Martin, E.M., Nixon, H., Pitrak, D.L., Weddington, W., risk for symptomatic decline. Neurology, 82(23), 2055- Rains, N.A., Nunnally G.,Grbes S., et al. (2007) 2062. Characteristics of prospective memory deficits in HIV- Grassi, M.P., Clerici, F., Perin, C., Zocchetti, C., Borella, seropositive substancedependent individuals: M., Cargnel, A., Mangoni, A. (1995). HIV infection and Preliminary observation, Journal of Clinical and drug use: influence on cognitive function. AIDS. 9, 165- Experimental Neuropsychology 29: 496–504. 170. Mondini S., Mapelli D., Vestri A., Arcara G., & Bisiacchi Goedert, J. (1995). Mortality in haemophilia. Lancet 346, P.S. (2003). Esame Neuropsicologico Breve. Cortina 1425–1426.doi:10.1016/ S0140-6736(95)92440-X Raffaello, Torino, Italy Heaton, R. K., Grant, I., Butters, N., White, D. A., Kirson, Reger, M., Welsh, R., Razani, J., Martin, D.J., & Boone, D., Atkinson, J. H., McCutchan, J.A., et al. (1995). The K.B. (2002) A meta-analysis of the neuropsychological HNRC 500: Neuropsychology of HIV infection at sequelae of HIV infection. Journal of the International different disease stages. Journal of the International Neuropsychology Society, 8:410–424. Neuropsychological Society, 1: 231–251. Rey, A. (1964) L’Examen clinique en psycologie. (The Heaton, R.K., Clifford, D.B., Franklin, D.R. Jr, Woods, clinical exam in psychology.) Presses Universitaires de S.P., Ake, C., Vaida, F., Ellis, R.J., et al. (2010). HIV France, Paris associated neurocognitive disorders persist in the era of Riedel, R.R., Helmstaedter, C., Bülau, P., Durwen, H.F., Brackmann, H., Fimmers, R., Clarenbach, P., et al. 387 (1992). Early signs of cognitive deficits among human Language and Information). Stanford; New York: immunodeficiency virus-positive haemophiliacs. Acta Cambridge University Press. Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 85: 321–326. Woods, P.S., Iudicello, J.E., Moran L.M., Carey, C.L., Riva, S., Cutica, I., & Pravettoni, G. (2014). Is there Dawson, M.S., Grant, I. and The HIV Neurobehavioral evidence for neurocognitive dysfunctions in patients Research Center (HNRC) (2008). HIV-associated with postnatal HIV infection? A review on the cohort of Prospective Memory Impairment Increases Risk of hemophilia patients. Frontiers in human neuroscience, Dependence in Everyday Functioning, 8. Neuropsychology, 22, 110–117 Rondanelli M, Caselli D, Arico M, Maccabruni, A., Magnani, B., Bacchella, L., De Stefano, A., et al. (2002). Insulin-like growth factor I (IGF-I) and IGF-binding protein 3 response to growth hormone is impaired in HIV-infected children. AIDS Research and Human Retroviruses, 18:331–339. Seider T.R., Assawin Gongvatana X.L., Devlin, K.N., de la Monte S.,M., Chasman, J.D., Yan, P., Tashima, K.T., et al. (2014). Verbal memory declines more rapidly with age in HIV infected versus uninfected adults. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 36:4, 356- 367 Selnes, O. A., Miller, E., McArthur, J., Gordon, B., Munoz, A., Sheridan, K., ... & Saah, A. J. (1990). HIV‐1 infection No evidence of cognitive decline during the asymptomatic stages. Neurology, 40(2), 204-204. Shimizu, S.M., Chow, D.C., Valcour, V., Masaki, K., Nakamoto, B., Kallianpur, K.J., & Shikuma, C. (2011) The impact of depressive symptoms on neuropsychological performance tests in HIV-infected individuals: A study of the Hawaii aging with HIV cohort. World Journal of AIDS, 1:139–145. Tombaugh Nichols, S., Mahoney, E.M., Sirois, P.A., Bordeaux, J.D., Stehbens, J.A., Loveland, K.A., Amodei, N., et al., (2000). HIV-Associated Changes in Adaptive, Emotional, and Behavioral Functioning in Children and Adolescents With Haemophilia: Results From the Haemophilia Growth and Development Study. Journal of Paediatric Psychology, 25, 545-556. Turnbull, O., Saling, M. M., Kaplan-Solms, K., Cohn, R., & Schoub, B. (1991). Neuropsychological deficit in haemophiliacs with human immunodeficiency virus. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 54(2), 175-177. Van Rossum, A.M., Gaakeer, M.I., Verweel, S., Hartwig, N.G., Wolfs, T.F., Geelen, S.P., Lamberts, S.W., et al. (2003). Endocrinologic and immunologic factors associated with recovery of growth in children with human immunodeficiency virus type 1 infection treated with protease inhibitors. Pediatric Infectious Disease Journal, 22:70–76. Watkins, J.M., Cool, V.A., Usner, D., Stehbens, J.A., Nichols, S., Loveland, K.A., Bordeaux, J.D., et al (2000). Attention in HIV-infected children: results from the Hemophilia Growth and Development Study. Journal of the International Neuropsychology Society, 6:443-454. Wechsler, S. (1995). The semantic basis of argument structure. CSLI Publications (Center for the Study of 388