=Paper= {{Paper |id=Vol-1419/paper0064 |storemode=property |title=Body Representation and Spatial Abilities of Preterm Low Birth Weight Preschool Children |pdfUrl=https://ceur-ws.org/Vol-1419/paper0064.pdf |volume=Vol-1419 |dblpUrl=https://dblp.org/rec/conf/eapcogsci/LabadiGB15 }} ==Body Representation and Spatial Abilities of Preterm Low Birth Weight Preschool Children== https://ceur-ws.org/Vol-1419/paper0064.pdf
    Body representation and spatial abilities of preterm low birth weight preschool
                                      children
                                           Beatrix Lábadi (labadi.beatrix@pte.hu)
                          Institute of Psychology, University of Pécs, Ifjúság u. 6. Pécs, 7627 Hungary
                                                           Enikő Györkő
                                       Institute of Psychology, University of Pécs, Hungary
                                                               Anna Beke
                      Obstetrics and Gynaecology Clinic No.1, Semmelweis University, Budapest, Hungary


                           Abstract                                      Recently an extensive interest has been shown about the role
                                                                         of bodily experience in cognitive processes. However, there
  The goal of the present study was to investigate the
  relationship between spatial ability and development of body           has been a widespread confusion about the nature of the
  representation in preterm low birth weight preschool four-             mental representation of body (Gallagher, 1986) mostly due
  year-old children without neurological deficit and typically           to the variability of existing taxonomies and models.
  developing children who were matched by IQ and                         According to the growing consensus in the field of
  chronological age. Our findings indicate that children born            neuropsychology       (Sirigu,    Grafman,     Bressler,   &
  prematurely with a normal cognitive level may have specific            Sunderland, 1991) as well as in developmental psychology
  difficulties in all levels of body representation which may be
  associated with the spatial language production. These finding
                                                                         (Slaughter & Heron, 2004) the three-level model is
  are relevant for understanding the qualitative aspects of body         supported which distinguishes (a) a sensori-motor
  representation and provide practical consequences for early            representation of the body; (b) a visual-spatial body
  intervention of children born prematurely.                             representation; and (c) a lexical-semantic representation of
  Keywords: body representation, spatial memory, spatial
                                                                         the body. The sensori-motor level consists of a short-term
  language, preterm born                                                 online representation of the body which is responsible for
                                                                         body movement and not accessible to consciousness. The
                       Introduction                                      visual-spatial representation consists of long-term and
                                                                         general knowledge about the body topography including the
Over the past decades, rapid development in the perinatal
                                                                         spatial localization of body parts. The lexical-semantic
and neonatal care has increased the survival rate of children
                                                                         representation involves the general knowledge of the body
who are born very premature, however, there are potential
                                                                         and its functions involving the naming of body parts or
risks for long-term morbidity. Indeed, children born preterm
                                                                         semantic knowledge about the body functions e.g. biological
with low birth weight often have neuromotor problems and
                                                                         background. The latter two levels of body representation are
are at risk for deficit in cognitive abilities in such
                                                                         accessible to consciousness.
neuropsychological domains as memory, attention,
                                                                         The available studies suggest that infants begin learning
executive function and language (Mikkola et al. 2005;
                                                                         about their bodies as newborns, but at that time they create
Marlow, Henessy, Bracewell & Wolke, 2007). The
                                                                         only a highly schematic representation of human body
longitudinal studies emphasize that preterm birth with low
                                                                         (Quinn & Eimas, 1998). The detailed visual-spatial
birth weight have long-lasting negative impact on the
                                                                         representations of human body emerge around 15- to 18
cognitive abilities and academic skills in school-aged
                                                                         months when they are capable to discriminate scrambled
children (Conley & Bennet, 2000). However, cognitive
                                                                         human body image from non-scrambled body image
impairments often cannot be detected clearly until these
                                                                         (Slaughter & Heron, 2004). Interestingly, visual-spatial
children begin school.
                                                                         representation of faces emerges earlier than body
Most psychological studies of cognitive functioning of
                                                                         representation; even newborns are able to discriminate
preterm children investigated the deficit in general domains
                                                                         human faces from scrambled faces (Johnson & Morton,
such as global intelligence, attention, perceptual-motor
                                                                         1991), infants are likely to be born with an innate schema of
functioning, executing functioning and memory. These
                                                                         human faces rather than a human body. The detailed visual-
assessments demonstrated that children born preterm have
                                                                         spatial representation becomes available from the second or
worse neuropsychological outcomes relative to typically
                                                                         third year of life when children begin to develop an explicit
developed children. Relatively small number of studies
                                                                         representation of the body and recognize the human shape
focused on the specific patterns of the cognitive abilities of
                                                                         with its distinctive configuration and spatial topography
preterm children such as spatial abilities, and none of these
                                                                         (Brownell, Nichols, Svetlova, Zerwas & Ramini, 2010).
studies investigated the specific impairment in body
                                                                         Based on the previous studies the visual-spatial
representation. Some studies reported that being born
                                                                         representation of the body is likely to derive from sensori-
prematurely with low birth weight is a risk for deficit in
                                                                         motor representation. This early bodily experience is the
spatial memory span and spatial working memory, as well
                                                                         root of the developing body representation, cognitive
as recognition memory (e.g., Georgieff & Nelson, 2002).


                                                                   401
abilities as well as the self. The body awareness is not a sort           The control group included children born at term having
of separated entity in the world; rather it is a relational             no history of perinatal problems. The full-term children with
between the body and either the physical and social                     typical developing characteristics (N=26) were born after 38
environment (Rochat, 2010). Even newborns begin to                      gestational weeks and individually matched with the
develop the implicit bodily self which is embedded in their             preterm sample for age, parents education and IQ
environment. Shortly after the birth neonates begin to learn            (Hungarian version of Brunet-Lezine Test). Full-term
the relation between their current capacities (e.g. motor               children were recruited from the local preschool selected by
skills, bodily constraints) and the environmental conditions.           teachers on the average level of the class.
But an important question is raised whether children who
are born very prematurely before 30 gestational weeks and               Materials and Procedure
spend their first month(s) in a sensory deprived environment              General procedures took place in a quiet lab. Each child
are able to develop a typical body representation.                      was tested individually. Three tasks were conducted to
                                                                        assess body representation of children and additional two
                      Present study                                     tasks evaluated children’s spatial abilities.
Children who were born prematurely and spent their first
weeks in incubator as a part of the intensive care have been            Tasks for body representation
frequently reported deficit in sensori-motor domain. The                Sensori-motor body representation - Fitting hands task.
lack of early physical contact with the world prevents them             Here, we investigated children’s ability to reason about their
from collecting experience from their own bodies which                  body size, and shape relative to the objective physical
might specifically impact on the later body knowledge and               world. In this task children were required to use their own
related cognitive abilities.                                            sensori-motor body representation while fitting their actions
This study was designed to answer specific questions about              to the visual-spatial patterns of the world. Nevertheless, our
different aspect of body representation in prematurely born             task included only the hands and it did not extend to the
children aged between 4 to 5 years. There are both practical            whole body. This task required the child to insert their hand
and theoretical reasons for addressing the questions of body            into one of two apertures to take out a toy from a box. The
representation. From a theoretical point of view, the                   apertures were placed on the top of a box and varied due to
research attempts to provide further evidence for the                   different visual-spatial patterns. To be able to solve the task,
childhood development of body representation and its                    the child required to recognize the spatial relationship of a
relation to spatial cognition. The practical aspect of the              visual pattern of the aperture and his/her own body
research is to provide deeper insights into the possible                properties and the bodily action. First, the child had to
deficit of body representation in preterm children for early            analyze the perceptual constraints of the apertures then
educational and rehabilitative intervention to improve the              compare them with their own hands properties. Finally, they
preterm children’s cognitive and behavioral outcome.                    were required to choose the correct aperture, orient and
                                                                        adjust their hands to the size, orientation and the shape of
                          Method                                        the aperture in order to insert one hand into the box.
                                                                        In this task we used a box (20 x 30 x 15 cm) with different
Participants                                                            interchangeable lids. The size of the apertures was adjusted
                                                                        to a typical 5-year-old child’s hand size. Each lid had two
We studied 31 preterm children aged between 4 and 5 years,              apertures which varied within three dimensions: size, shape
who were born before 30 gestational weeks (Mean: 27.93                  and orientation. The apertures were presented side by side
weeks, SD: 1.63; ranging from 25 to 30 gestational weeks)               on the lid and one of two apertures violated the physical
and their birth weight ranged from 600 g to 1680 g (Mean:               constraints, therefore children were prevented from inserting
1040g, SD: 241 g). Additional 12 children were discarded                their hands into this aperture, for example the aperture was
because of fussiness (N =4) or incomplete task performance              smaller than the child’s hand. Each child performed 9 trials
on more than 3 tasks (N=8). Preterm children were enrolled              (three per dimension) and the order of the stimuli was
via the Department of Pediatric Neurology at the Obstetrics             randomized.
and Gynaecology Clinic No.1 of Semmelweis University in                    The experiment was recorded and the tapes were time-
Budapest. Prior to the study children were assessed by a                coded by digital clock. The hand actions were also coded
clinical neuropsychologist and a pediatric neurologist who              and analyzed by two independent raters for the purpose of
ensured that children are within the normal range of                    assessing successful choice, reaction time and qualitative
intellectual abilities without neurological symptoms,                   analysis of hand laterality.
however they are in the lower part of the normal range as                  Visual-spatial body representation - Scrambled body task
the most of the very preterm children (< 30 gestational                    To investigate the visual-spatial body representation we
week). The inclusion criteria for preterm children were the             reproduced scrambled body images used by Slaughter,
following: (1) birth at a gestational age of 30 weeks or                Heron & Sim (2002). However, we modified them by using
younger; (2) no congenital abnormalities; (3) no measurable             friendlier children figures instead of adult pictures, and we
neurological deficit; (4) no retinopathy or prematurity; (5)            also changed the presentation method. We used a pairwise
no mental, intellectual disabilities.


                                                                  402
comparison method with two sets of human body pictures,                 but the same complexity provides good grounds for testing
one for typical body and another for scrambled body. Each               path type effects on spatial language use, which is not
set consisted of 6 six images, these were black and white               available in all languages - for example, English often uses
line drawings. The scrambled body set violated the typical              the same postpositions for static and goal relations.
canonical human body shape, for example legs attached to
the shoulders or arms attached to the hip. The pairings of
typical and scrambled figure pictures and the side of
presentations were randomized across children. The children
were asked to decide which picture showed a typical body.
We measured the correct responses and analyzed the typical
errors.
   Lexical- semantic body representation - Body part
localization task
   This task investigated children’s ability to locate their
body parts on themselves. The task was adopted from adult
studies examining the body representation deficits in adults
with focal brain damages. Children were asked to point to
their own body parts as the examiner named them. This task
was divided into two parts, one of which referred to the                  Fig. 1. Spatial layout in the spatial language task
naming of the head parts and the other requested naming the
other body parts (we determined 7 standard locations on the                In our study the spatial terms were elicited in an
head and 24 locations on the body). The whole procedure                 experimental space consisting of a 1.5 x 1.5 m matrix
was recorded and two independent trained raters coded the               involving a white mug divided into 16 identical squares
performance (within these categories: correct location,                 with black lines (Fig. 1). We also used five little wooden
different body part, refuse). Interraters reliability was .93.          chairs made for children with different tops representing
                                                                        animals (e.g. monkey) and fruits (e.g. pumpkin) as reference
Tasks for spatial abilities                                             objects. During the experiment these chairs were placed into
                                                                        the matrix, while children were requested to answer 3 types
   Spatial memory. The spatial memory task was adopted                  of questions encoding the path e.g. ’Where is the monkey?’
from the Hungarian Version of Snijders – Oomen                          (static); ’Where do I put the melon?’ (goal); ’Where do I
Nonverbal Intelligence Scale for Young Children. This                   take the apple from?’ (source). We tested the spatial term
subtest assesses the spatial location memory in young                   production within either egocentric or allocentric frame of
children. The participant is presented with a little house              reference. The experimenter was standing outside of the
shape made of paper with six or ten windows depending on                matrix and put the target object (chair) to different positions
the trials. The windows represent the hiding locations,                 related to either one or two other objects (chairs) or the child
which are displayed in three horizontal rows with 6                     depending on the spatial frame of reference. In the
windows, and L-shaped configuration with ten windows,                   allocentric situation the child stood outside of the matrix
where these extra four windows are added to one side of the             and he/she was required to respond to the experimenter’s
house. In each trial, the experimenter places a black kitten            questions referring to the relations among the objects (e.g.
made of paper into one of the windows then quickly closes               ‘The apple is next to the melon’). While in the egocentric
all windows and covered the place with a screen for 6 s.                situation child sat in the centre of the matrix on a chair and
When the screen is removed the children are immediately                 requested to answer the question from his/her egocentric
being requested to point the window where the kitten was                viewpoint (e.g. ‘The apple is next to me’). Altogether 12
hid. The number of the correct responses was computed for               postpositions were tested in each situation. Children’s
the analysis.                                                           scores were computed according to the number of correct
   Spatial language production                                          ‘spatial postposition’ productions.
   Hungarian language has many possibilities to encode
spatial relations: suffixes, postpositions, verbal prefixes and                                    Results
adverbs (Lukács, Pléh, & Racsmány, 2007). In this study we              Sensori-motor body representation - Fitting hands to a
focus on postpositions that are used to encode cognitively              visual form task. The hand actions were analyzed due to the
complex relations and postpositions providing cue to encode             number of correct choices and action duration.
the path type in three different forms according to the                 Correct insertion: For each trial, the correct attempts to
dynamic aspect of coding the location and the path. For                 insert the hand into the aperture were coded and calculated
each spatial relation, Hungarian has a static locative term,            as a correct choice, but it was only the first attempt that was
and two dynamic forms, one encoding the goal or end of the              counted. A repeated measures of ANOVA with visual-
path; and the other relates to the source or starting point of          spatial features (form, size and orientation) as the within
the path. All three types can be distinguished linguistically           subjects factor and groups (preterm and full-term) as the



                                                                  403
between subject factor was conducted on the scores of the              groups (F (1, 54) = 28.635 p <.000). Children born full-term
correct responses. No significant difference was found                 achieved more than twice as many scores as prematurely
between the groups (F (1, 50) = 1.198 p =ns.). But a                   born children did. We also found differences between the
significant effect emerged for visual-spatial feature (F (1,           conditions (allocentric and egocentric; F(1, 54) = 11.916
50) = 26.162, p < .000), namely, children tended to perform            p<.001). In both groups children performed poorer in the
better in trials of orientation than form or size trials. No           egocentric condition, they could use more postpositions
significant interaction between the group and visual-spatial           viewed from outside of the matrix.
features was found.                                                    Relationship between body representation and spatial
Time: we registered the overall duration of action, the time           abilities in preterm children. To determine whether preterm
from the appearance of the novel lid until the child took out          children’s body representations were related to spatial
the toy from the box. The incorrect choices were excluded              abilities Pearson correlations were calculated among these
from the analysis. Repeated measures of ANOVA (2 x 3 x                 variables with age partialled out from the calculation
3) were used to compare preterm and full-term children as              (Table1). Surprisingly, we have not found association
between subject variable (preterm vs. full-term) and trials as         between the performance in scrambled body task and the
well as visual-spatial features (shape, orientation and size)          spatial variables. However, positive association was found
as within subject variables. We found significant differences          between the performance in allocentric spatial language and
between the groups for the overall reaction time (F (1, 50) =          body localization test (r = .680, p <.21); whereas the
7.609, p <.009). Preterm children (M= 3.93, SE =.198) spent            egocentric spatial language showed negative correlation
more time to solve this problem than full-term children                with the meantime of hand action (r = -.606, p<.048).
(M=3.02, SE = .193). We also found differences between                 Furthermore, the egocentric spatial language showed
the visual-spatial categories (F (2,50) = 24.433, p<.000),             positive association with the mean correct choices in the
where the pairwise comparison showed that the orientation              hand task (r = .692, p<.018). In contrast, no significant
category (M=2.409, SE =.094) of the visual-spatial pattern             association was found between the performance in spatial
of the aperture differed either from the shape (M = 4.005,             memory and different levels of body representation.
SE = .205) or the size (M= 3.787, SE = .236) category.
Significant interaction was found among group, trial and                  Table 1 Intercorrelation among body representation
visual-spatial features (F (4, 50) =3.409, p <.011).                       and spatial ability measures in preterm children
Visual-spatial body representation - Scrambled body task
We compared the performance between the preterm and                                         Spatial             Spatial           Spatial
full-term group based on the total scores. The analysis                                   language            language           memory
                                                                                          Allocentric          Egocentric
revealed significant differences (F (1, 60) = 4.901; p< .031).
                                                                   Scrambled body             .404                .369               .197
The premature children’s performances were poorer (M=
5.04, SD= .79) than those of the control peers (M= 5.92,           Body part                  .680*               .103               .394
SD= .37). These differences can be described by the                localization
difficulties in the discrimination of the limbs.                   Whole body
Body part localization. One-way analysis of variance               Hand fitting               .027               .692*               .119
                                                                   Correct choices
(ANOVA) was conducted as appropriate on the measures of
both dimensions of body representation such as the head and        Hand fitting                 .191              -.606*             -.226
the whole body. Significant effect for both dimensions             task
emerged, and full-term children showed better performance            Note df varied from 24-29 depending on the number of children completing
either for head dimension (F (1, 47) = 11.609; p<.001) and           a given task of overall sample
                                                                        *p < .05
for the whole body dimension (F(1, 47) = 28.975; p<.000).
Though, the scores of head were near the ceiling in both
                                                                       To determine to what body representation levels are related,
groups (preterm: M =6.32, SD=.72; full-term: M= 6.94,
                                                                       Pearson correlation were computed controlling for age (in
SD=.24). Children in the full-term group (M=19.17,
                                                                       months) in the whole sample. The correlations were
SD=2.40) exhibited higher performance than their preterm
                                                                       calculated among each score of performance in each body
peers (M=14.22, SD = 3.27).
                                                                       representation tasks:         scrambled body, body part
Spatial memory. Delayed recall score of the two groups
                                                                       localization, fitting hands. Significant correlation (see Table
were analyzed by a one-way analysis of variance
                                                                       5) were observed between the performance of scrambled
(ANOVA). The preterm children obtained a lower score (M
                                                                       body task and performance in both body localization tasks
= 6.70 SD = 2.57) than did the control group (M=7.84, SD =
                                                                       (for head r =.427, p <.04; r =.471, p<.04; for whole body r
2.22), but no significant difference was found (F (1, 56) =
                                                                       =.471 p<.02) as well as the scores of correct choices in hand
3.122, p= .08).
                                                                       fitting task (r=.427, p <.04). None of the variables of hand
Spatial language. A repeated measures of ANOVA (Type
                                                                       fitting task related to the outcome of other body knowledge
III) for spatial language (egocentric, allocentric) as within
                                                                       variables. The variables of hand fitting task was associated
subjects factor and for groups as between subjects factor
                                                                       only with each other, namely, the means of the correct
was conducted. Significant effects were revealed for the



                                                                 404
responses negatively correlated with the mean time of                   because even toddlers are capable to discriminate the
preadjustment (r= -.545, p<.007).                                       scrambled body from the typical body (Slaughter et al.,
                                                                        2003). In the other body topography (body part localization)
                        Discussion                                      task children were asked to point body parts by name. The
Early development of body representation comparing                      findings showed that full-term children are superior. The
preterm and full-term children was investigated in the                  preterm children’s poor performances remind us of the
current study. We reported evidence that children born very             neuropsychological deficit at the level of lexical-semantic
prematurely, without major neurological deficits and with a             body representation, patients (autotopagnosia) with damage
normal cognitive level, have specific difficulties in most of           to the left parietal area have difficulties to localize their own
the body related tasks evaluated at 4 years of age. We                  body parts when the examiner names them, but they can
further found that the reduced performance in body                      identify parts of inanimate objects (Guariglia et al., 2002).
representation is related to spatial language, but not to the           These results suggest dissociation between the topographic
spatial memory within the preterm group.                                representation and semantic representation of their own
Relative to full-term controls, preterm children in the                 body. Nevertheless, in our preterm sample we did not find
present study showed reduced performance at all levels of               that the semantic representation is dissociated from the
body knowledge. However, in the task of fitting hand,                   visuospatial representation, because the children’s
where the children are requested to rely on their sensori-              performance of body part localization correlated with the
motor body representation, both groups are equally good at              performance score in scrambled body task (for whole body r
scaling their reaching action to size, form and orientation of          =.405 p < .04). Notably, the impairments in movement-
the aperture. By contrast, previous findings of younger                 related representations (hand fitting) of one’s body did not
children (1.5 - 2 years) showed a poor performance in a                 correlate with the two other representational levels either in
similar scaling task (Ishak and Adolph, 2008), where                    the preterm sample or in the whole sample. Such findings
toddlers frequently attempted to fit their hands into the               suggest that sensori-motor body representation might be a
impossibly small holes. As Brownell et al (2010) suggested              distinct aspect of the body representation.
the awareness of body to one’s own body size begins to                  The last question we addressed refers to the possible
emerge in the second year of life but in very limited ways              relationship between the body-related representations and
and continues developing over the childhood.                            spatial abilities in preterm children. Our data suggested that
In contrast to our prediction, significant differences were not         the production of spatial language from two different spatial
found between the preterm and full-term groups regarding                viewpoints, using allocentric vs. egocentric frame of
their performance scores. But they differed in reaction time;           reference, was associated with the body representations,
preterm children solve this task slower than the control. As            namely the preterm children who showed better
Milner and Goodale (1995) noted the reaction time of a                  performance in the body location tasks obtained better
particular hand action refers to the transformation speed of            outcomes of spatial language production (within the
the visuospatial information into motor execution. This                 allocentric reference). Moreover, the performance of hand
process is the function of the dorsal stream, and the longer            fitting task also correlated with the spatial language
reaction time in preterm children suggests an impairment of             productions. It seems that the body might play an important
dorsal system functioning. This finding is consistent with              role in the spatial representation as the popular theory of
other studies (Braddick, Atkinson and Wattam-Bell, 2003,                embodiment suggested. The body is used as a sort of
Taylor, Jakobson, Maurer and Lewis, 2009) suggesting the                reference frame (head-feet, front-back, left-right) which is
increased vulnerability of the dorsal stream in children born           mapped onto the embodied objects, for example ‘I am
prematurely. The different amount of time in transforming               behind the melon’ (Lakoff and Johnson, 1999). However,
the visuospatial information into execution is likely to                the speaker can use a viewer-centered (egocentric or deictic)
account for the group differences. As we observed, preterm              or object-centered (allocentric or intrinsic) frame of
children needed more time to take out the toy from the box              reference, and using the allocentric rather than egocentric
through the aperture hole because they had not adjusted                 spatial reference frame to describe the spatial relations can
their hand to the visuospatial patterns in advance.                     refer to the objective and viewpoint independent approach
To study children’s knowledge of body topography we                     of the world. In fact, preterm children’s production of
created two age-appropriate modifications of previously                 egocentric spatial language correlated with the movement-
used tasks, we investigated the children’s topographic body             related body representation. We propose children who use
knowledge using typical human bodies versus scrambled                   an egocentric view to describe the scene are involved
bodies portrayed in various postures. Preterm children                  bodily. An impaired sensori-motor body representation is
tended more frequently to fail to discriminate the canonical            not allowed to provide a stable egocentric reference point to
body posture from the scrambled body, especially in that                determine the locations.
case when the arms and legs were interchanged. Our
findings suggest that children aged 4 years are able to                                           Conclusion
discriminate the canonical human body confidently                       This study is the first to examine the different levels of body
regardless of the body posture. This result is not surprising,          knowledge associated with some spatial abilities in children



                                                                  405
born very prematurely. Relative to full-term sample, preterm             Brownell, C., Zerwas, S., Ramini, G. (2007). “So big”: The
children showed reduced performance in all levels of body                  development of body self-awareness in toddlers. Child
representation which are associated to the production of                   Development, 78, 1426-1440.
spatial language.                                                        Brownell, C. A., Nichols, S. R., Svetlova, M., Zerwas, S.,
There were theoretical and practical reasons for questioning               Ramini, G. (2010). The head bone’s connected to the neck
this issue. From a theoretical point of view only limited                  bone: When do toddlers represent their own body
number of studies investigated the possible relationship                   topography? Child Development, 81, (3) 797-810.
between the body representation and spatial cognition, and               Conley, D., & Bennett, N. G. (2000). Is biology destiny?
none of them focused on the specific developmental risks of                Birth weight and life chances. American Sociological
preterm birth. Nevertheless, the embodiment theory                         Review, 65, 458–468.
emphasizes the body experience as a ground of many                       Gallagher, S. (1986). Body Image and Body Schema: A
different psychological functions, such as emotions and                    Conceptual Clarification, Journal of Mind and Behavior
cognition. As Esther Thelen (2000) claimed the cognitive                   7, 541-554.
processes emerge from the bodily experience as someone is                K. E., & Lin, G. C. (2008). Perceiving affordances for
interacting with the world and this experience is constrained              fitting through apertures. Journal of Experimental
by the particular motor and perceptual capabilities. On the                Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 34,
other hand, the practical reason of this study is to provide               1501-1514.
indications for early intervention of cognitive abilities based          Marlow, N., Henessy, E. M., Bracewell, M. A., Wolke, D.
on the body knowledge that is thought to be a potential                    (2007). Motor and executive function at 6 years of age
predictor of learning disabilities. Our findings suggest that              after extremely preterm birth. Pediatrics, 120, 793-804.
the early body experience is very important for the later                Mikkola, K., Ritari, N, Tommiska, V., Salokorpi, T.,
development, because infants discover the world through                    Lehtonen, L. et al. (2005). Neurodevelopmental outcome
their bodies, e.g. how their bodies move in space, how their               at 5 years of age of a national cohort of extremely low
bodies relate to the objects in the world (Adolph and Berger,              birth weight nfants who were born in 1996–1997.
2006); while they are capable to differentiate their bodies                Pediatrics 116. 1391 -1400.
form the physical world from the first year of the life.                 Moore, C. (2007). Understanding self and others in the
Therefore, from a practical view, the early intervention to                second year. In. C. Brownell, C. Kopp (Eds.) Transitions
improve these children’s body knowledge at all levels is                   in early socioemotional development: The toddler years.
worthy of consideration.                                                   New York: Guilford, 43-65.
  Limitations. Our results must be viewed with caution for               Quinn, P. C., Eimas, P. D. (2000). The emergence of
some reasons. First, we did not control the birth weight                   category representation during infancy: Are separated and
relative to gestational weeks, our preterm sample involved                 conceptual process required? Journal of Cognition and
children with relatively wide range (600 g to 1680 g)                      Development, 1, 55-61.
however previous studies showed that the birth weight (as a              Quinn, P. C., Eimas, P. D. (1998). Evidence for a global
degree of prematurity) is a good predictor of the future                   categorical representation of humans by young infants.
cognitive abilities. Second, it is not clear whether the results           Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 69, 151-174.
of preterm children are specific to body representations or              Rochat, P. (2010). The innate sense of the body develops to
the problem in body representation itself is a consequence                 become a public affair by 2-3 years. Neuropsychologia,
of prior deficit. The future work should explore how the                   48. 738-745.
body representation relates to prematurity specifically.                 Rose, S. A., & Feldman, J. F. (1996). Memory and
                                                                           processing speed in preterm children at 11 years: A
                   Acknowledgments                                         comparison with full-terms. Child Development, 67,
This research was supported by OTKA (PD – 109597)                          2005–2021.
research grant.                                                          Sirigu, A., Grafman, J., Bressler, K., Sunderland, T. (1991).
                                                                           Multiple representations contribute to body knowledge
                                                                           processing. Brain, 114, 629-642.
                        References                                       Slaughter, V., Heron, M. (2004). Origin and early
Adolph, K. E. & Berger, S. A. (2006). Motor development.                   development of human body knowledge. Monographs of
  In W. Damon & R. Lerner (Series Eds.) & D. Kuhn & R.                     the Society for Research in Child Development, 69 (2,
  S. Siegler (Vol. Eds.) Handbook of child psychology: Vol                 Serial No. 276)
  2: Cognition, perception, and language (6th ed.) New                   Slaughter, V., Heron, M. & Sim, (2002). Development of
  York: Wiley, 161-213.                                                    preferences for the human body shape in infancy,
Braddick , O. Atkinson, J., Wattam-Bell, J. (2003). Normal                 Cognition 85, B71–B81.
  and anomalous development of visual motion processing:                 Taylor, N. M., Jakobson, L. S., Maurer, D., Lewis, T. L.
  Motion coherence and ‘dorsal-stream vulnerability’                       (2009). Differential vulnetability of global motion, global
  Neuropsychologia, 41, 1769-1784.                                         form, and biological motion processing in full-term and
                                                                           preterm children. Neuropsychologia, 2766-2778.



                                                                   406