=Paper=
{{Paper
|id=Vol-2323/SKI-Canada-2019-7-1-5
|storemode=property
|title=Web Maps for Global Data Visualization: Does Mercator Matter?
|pdfUrl=https://ceur-ws.org/Vol-2323/SKI-Canada-2019-7-1-5.pdf
|volume=Vol-2323
|authors=Sam Lumley,Renee Sieber
}}
==Web Maps for Global Data Visualization: Does Mercator Matter?==
Spatial Knowledge and Information Canada, 2019, 7(1), 5 Web Maps for Global Data Visualization: Does Mercator Matter? SAM LUMLEY R ENEE SIEBER Department of Geography Department of Geography School of Environment School of Environment McGill University McGill University sam.lumley@mail.mcgill.ca renee.sieber@mcgill.ca ABSTRACT 1. Introduction 1.1 Web maps for data visualization The Mercator projection has become a Web map visualization describes the standard across web mapping platforms, but interactive display of geographic has long been considered inappropriate for information on a computer-based map global data display due to its distortion of high (Kraak & Brown, 2014). By giving users an latitude areas. With the ever-rising popularity of web maps, the Mercator projection has seen intuitive schema for navigation, web maps a resurgence in its use for spatial data represent a popular communication tool for visualizations. In this study we investigated sharing spatial information (Elwood, 2011; the implications of the area distortion effects Johnson & Sieber, 2012). Further, the of the Mercator projection for public data development of map tiling services over the interpretation. We recruited 120 participants past decade has dramatically reduced the via Amazon’s Mechanical Turk platform to computational demands for data retrieval complete an online survey assessing their and display (Haklay, Singleton, & Parker, ability to identify and account for the 2008), enabling user-friendly interaction distortion effects. Participants were asked to and serving the information seeking estimate the areas covered by five colored Mantra: “overview first, details on demand” regions on a global map, having been split into (Shneiderman, 1996). a control group using an equal-area projection and a treatment group using a Mercator As a result, there is a growing adoption of projection. On average, participants did not web mapping applications, such as Google discount for the projection and their data Maps, OpenLayers and Mapbox APIs, in interpretation differed between the two public data portals and interactive maps conditions as a result. Our findings provide an (Batty, Hudson-Smith, Milton, & Crooks, empirical basis for the distortion effects of the 2010). This resurgence demands further Mercator projection currently used in web research into the perceptual implications of maps, and further implicate its the Mercator projection’s area distortions. appropriateness for displaying global data. More broadly, they introduce experimental Understanding how such representational methods for research exploring cartographic features influence public data interpretation biases in non-expert groups. represents a critical issue in GIScience, and will be key to improving cartographic communication more generally. 1.2 Mercator in web maps The Mercator projection has become the standard across web mapping applications (Battersby, Finn, Usery, & Yamamoto, 2014). The preservation of angles (conformality) and universally upward 2 Web Maps for Global Data Visualization: Does Mercator Matter? pointing north (cylindricality) make it scale cognitive maps. Their results from 194 ideally suited for street mapping services student participants’ area estimations of (Strebe, 2012). The variant used in web world regions suggested that projection mapping represents the earth as a square at choice had a lower-than-expected impact on its lowest zoom level by truncating each pole cognitive maps, a finding further explicated by 5°. These properties come at the expense in a follow up review by Battersby et al. of area distortions that increase from the (2014). Aside from a study on map equator to the poles. projection preferences (Šavrič, Jenny, White, & Strebe, 2015), most recent While mapping platforms provide a experimental research on map projections powerful and convenient tool for data has been focused on academic or expert visualization, past research has shown that populations. As the number of web mapping even experienced users can struggle to applications used to display scientific data compensate for distortions when making rises, it will be increasingly important to on-the-spot judgements (Downs & Liben, understand the implications of projection 1991; MacEachren, 2004). The “Mercator choices in digital interfaces for non-expert Effect” predicts that people overemphasize audiences (Nocke, Flechsig, & Bohm, 2007; the importance of the enlarged high latitude Slocum et al., 2001), a primary objective of regions (Saarinen, 1988), which can lead to the present study. an inaccurate interpretation of any global data being overlaid. Critical geographers 1.3 The present study further argue that the distortion and This study assesses the influence of the orientation effects have served to reinforce Mercator projection on area estimation and European colonialism (Harpold, 1999), and data interpretation in non-expert audiences. more recently new forms of ‘digital Specifically, we were interested in whether imperialism’ (Farman, 2010). people identify the distortions, and if they do, how able they are to account for them. For this reason, the Mercator projection has This question was addressed through an long been renounced for use in scientific online experimental survey exploring visualization on the grounds that its area impacts on area-based judgements about distortions mislead map readers (Robinson, global geospatial data. To this end, we 1966). Despite this turbulent history and advanced two hypotheses: (1) individuals recent resurgence, there are still relatively making on-the-fly judgements about spatial few empirical studies investigating the data presented on a map are unlikely to cognitive implications of map projections identify or correct for projection distortions for data display (Battersby et al., 2014). and; (2) even if individuals are aware of the Further, it is unclear whether past results distortions, they will struggle to accurately remain relevant (Montello, Waller, Hegarty, convert back to the corresponding areas. & Richardson, 2004), particularly in light of recent mapping technologies (Lapon, Ooms, 2. Methods and Data: & Maeyer, 2017). Digital interfaces offer new opportunities and new modalities 2.1 Participants through which people can engage with Participants (N = 120) were recruited using spatial data (Haklay et al., 2008). The Amazon’s Mechanical Turk online hiring resulting shifts in use warrant further platform (Amazon, 2014). Mechanical Turk investigation. is a well-established recruitment tool used widely in social science research (Berinsky A recent body of research has begun to et al., 2012; Litman et al., 2017), and has explore these implications. Notably, been implemented successfully in (Battersby & Montello, 2009) investigated cartographic research more recently (e.g. the influence of map projections on global- Retchless & Brewer, 2015; Šavrič et al., 2015). All of our respondents were adults Web Maps for Global Data Visualization: Does Mercator Matter? 3 living in the United States and participated earth’s surface. This construction through a Qualtrics online survey. Our corresponded closely enough to a relatable sample had a mean self-reported age of 35 real-world example, but was abstract years (SD = 13.0), with 29% female and 42% enough for participants to engage without with a bachelor’s degree as their highest strong prior perceptions influencing their attained level of education. Participants responses (a common problem encountered were offered $1.00 for completing the during our pilot surveys which used a survey, plus a $0.50 performance-based temperature labelling scheme). Participants bonus. After eliminating responses with were asked to estimate the total area incomplete or unusable answers, we covered by each of the five pollutants. retained 113 valid responses. Further interpretation of the data was evaluated by asking respondents to choose 2.2 Design which of two particular colored pollutants Participants were randomly assigned to one they perceived to be a greater threat to the of two conditions: a treatment condition earth. using a Mercator version of the map (N = 60) and a control condition using an equal- Participants were next given a short area (Lambert cylindrical) version (N = 53). explanation of how different projections The control map projection was chosen unavoidably distort areas and/or shapes because areas could be compared at face- displayed on maps. After this briefing, it was value across the image, while also being a hoped that some participants would decide commonly used projection (Šavrič et al., that their previous area judgements had 2015). been be influenced by the projection they had been given. They were then shown a The data used in the map visualizations was blank version of both projections and asked derived from a global temperature dataset which one they thought was more suitable downloaded from the University of East for an area estimation task, and given the Anglia Climatic Research Unit’s website option to alter their original estimates in (Jones, New, Parker, Martin, & Rigor, light of the briefing. Participants in the 1999). The data was interpolated and color- treatment condition changing their quantized to produce five lateral regions estimations would provide evidence that that emphasized the Mercator Effect, and they had identified and attempted to then overlaid on a country outline map. account for the Mercator Effect. Figure 1.0 shows a greyscale version the two map projections given to participants. We used the Image Color Summarizer tool (Krzywinski, 2016) to calculate the face- value areas for each shaded region, measured as a percentage of the entire image, such that the face-value areas for the control map represented the undistorted area values. 2.3 Procedure To investigate the effects of projection choice on data interpretation, we designed an area estimation and threat perception task. Participants were shown a global-scale map with categorical data displayed (Figure 1.0), which they were told represented the presence of five different pollutants over the 4 Web Maps for Global Data Visualization: Does Mercator Matter? B 10.2 7.2 17.7 15.0 C 13.3 15.5 20.1 23 D 26.7 27.5 18.2 19. E 42.3 45.9 20.7 26.0 3.2 Data interpretation and map suitability A chi-square test of independence was used to examine the relationship between data interpretation and projection choice. The difference between conditions was significant, 𝜒2 (1, N = 113) = 13.58, p < 0.01. In particular, 17% of participants in the equal-area condition (N = 53) perceived pollutant A to be a greater threat than pollutant E, compared to 50% in the Mercator condition (N = 60). A chi-square test was used to test for differences in the answers to the map suitability questions. No Figure 1.0: The equal-area (top) and Mercator significant difference was found between (bottom) data visualizations given to conditions; participants did not judge one participants via an online survey. projection to be better than the other for making area judgements. 3. Results 3.1 Area estimation 4. Conclusion We tested for the effects of projection type The results from the area estimation and using independent-samples t-tests to data interpretation tasks indicated that compare the equal-area and Mercator participants’ judgements were significantly conditions across the five area estimations affected by the choice of projection. made by participants. We found a Specifically, participants took the maps at significant difference across all the regions. face-value and interpreted the data Specifically, participants overestimated the accordingly. This result was corroborated by areas which had been enlarged by the responses to follow-up questions, which Mercator projection, in line with face-value suggested that participants identified the area judgements, as shown in Table 1.0. Mercator projection as being equally Similarly, the control condition estimates appropriate to the control projection for corresponded closely with the face-value area estimation tasks, as well as the fact that measurements for the equal-area projection. they chose not to adjust their answers to the Surprisingly, the answers to the second area second part of the survey. estimation question did not differ significantly from the original answers; Further work would be necessary to refine while some participants in both categories the methods used in this study. It is possible chose to alter their answers, most stuck with that some of the documented effects could their original estimates. have been observed if participants had not fully understood the wording of the Table 1.0: Comparison of the mean estimate and questions. Additionally, there were several face-value proportions (%) across conditions. unaddressed confounds between the two Equal-area Mercator conditions which could have contributed Mean Face- Mean Face- towards the observed differences, such as Region estimate value estimate value A 7.5 4.0 23.3 16.2 the image dimensions and differences in Web Maps for Global Data Visualization: Does Mercator Matter? 5 granularity between the maps which arose Mapping. Cartographica: The due to scaling deformations. Despite these International Journal for Geographic limitations, the central result, that the Information and Geovisualization, 49(2), Mercator projection biases global data 85–101. https://doi.org/10.3138/carto.49.2.2313 interpretation, has concrete implications for Battersby, S. E., & Montello, D. R. (2009). Area geovisualization and GIScience research. estimation of world regions and the projection of the global-scale cognitive This study has provided empirical evidence map. Annals of the Association of for the Mercator Effect in web maps. We American Geographers, 99(2), 273–291. found that individuals were unlikely or Batty, M., Hudson-Smith, A., Milton, R., & unable to identify and re-project area data Crooks, A. (2010). Map mashups, Web 2.0 displayed on a Mercator projection to and the GIS revolution. Annals of GIS, corresponding areas on the earth’s surface, 16(1), 1–13. corroborating past research (Monmonier, https://doi.org/10.1080/19475681003700 831 1996; Robinson, 1966). Our framing of the Downs, R. M., & Liben, L. S. (1991). The tasks deliberately pointed towards the Development of Expertise in Geography: A potential for misinterpretation of data in Cognitive-Developmental Approach to real-world decision-making scenarios. More Geographic Education. Annals of the broadly, the results emphasize the strong Association of American Geographers, influence of cartographic design on public 81(2), 304–327. interpretation of geographic information. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467- Further work should critically assess efforts 8306.1991.tb01692.x to address the Mercator effect in web maps, Elwood, S. (2011). Geographic information such as the inclusion of gridlines, alternative science: Visualization, visual methods, and the geoweb. Progress in Human web mapping projections and adaptive Geography, 35(3), 401–408. maps (Jenny, 2012). Further GIScience Farman, J. (2010). Mapping the digital empire: research can continue to broaden our Google Earth and the process of understanding of the complex relationships postmodern cartography. New Media & between visual representation and Society, 12(6), 869–888. perception of geospatial information. https://doi.org/10.1177/146144480935090 0 Acknowledgements Haklay, M., Singleton, A., & Parker, C. (2008). Web Mapping 2.0: The Neogeography of the GeoWeb. Geography Compass, 2(6), This study was completed in the 2011–2039. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749- Department of Geography at McGill 8198.2008.00167.x University under the supervision of Harpold, T. (1999). Dark continents: A critique Professor Renee Sieber and Dr. Jin Xing, of Internet metageographies. Postmodern both of whom I thank for their support and Culture, 9(2). guidance. We also thank the two anonymous Jenny, B. (2012). Adaptive composite map reviewers for their helpful comments on projections. IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics, earlier drafts of the manuscript. 18(12), 2575–2582. Johnson, P. A., & Sieber, R. E. (2012). Motivations driving government adoption References of the Geoweb. GeoJournal, 77(5), 667– 680. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10708-011- Amazon. (2014). Amazon Mechanical Turk - 9416-8 Welcome. Retrieved October 21, 2017, from Jones, P. D., New, M., Parker, D. E., Martin, S., https://www.mturk.com/mturk/welcome & Rigor, I. G. (1999). Surface air Battersby, S. E., Finn, M. P., Usery, E. L., & temperature and its changes over the past Yamamoto, K. H. (2014). Implications of 150 years. Reviews of Geophysics, 37(2), Web Mercator and Its Use in Online 173–199. https://doi.org/10.1029/1999RG900002 6 Web Maps for Global Data Visualization: Does Mercator Matter? Kraak, J.-M., & Brown, A. (2014). Web Retchless, D. P., & Brewer, C. A. (2015). cartography. CRC Press. Guidance for representing uncertainty on Krzywinski, M. (2016). Image Color Summarizer global temperature change maps. - RGB and HSV Image Statistics. Retrieved International Journal of Climatology, December 14, 2017, from 36(3), 1143–1159. http://mkweb.bcgsc.ca/color- https://doi.org/10.1002/joc.4408 summarizer/?analyze Robinson, A. H. (1966). The look of maps: An Lapon, L., Ooms, K., & Maeyer, P. D. (2017). The examination of cartography design. Influence of the Web Mercator Projection University of Wisconsin Press. on the Global-Scale Cognitive Map of Web Saarinen, T. F. (1988). Centering of mental maps Map Users. In Proceedings of Workshops of the world. National Geographic and Posters at the 13th International Research, 4(1), 112–127. Conference on Spatial Information Theory Šavrič, B., Jenny, B., White, D., & Strebe, D. R. (COSIT 2017) (pp. 79–82). Springer, Cham. (2015). User preferences for world map https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-63946- projections. Cartography and Geographic 8_17 Information Science, 42(5), 398–409. MacEachren, A. M. (2004). How Maps Work: https://doi.org/10.1080/15230406.2015.10 Representation, Visualization, and Design. 14425 Guilford Press. Shneiderman, B. (1996). The eyes have it: A task Monmonier, M. (1996). How to lie with maps. by data type taxonomy for information University of Chicago Press. visualizations. In Visual Languages, 1996. Montello, D. R., Waller, D., Hegarty, M., & Proceedings., IEEE Symposium on (pp. Richardson, A. E. (2004). Spatial memory 336–343). IEEE. of real environments, virtual environments, Slocum, T. A., Blok, C., Jiang, B., Koussoulakou, and maps. Human Spatial Memory: A., Montello, D. R., Fuhrmann, S., & Remembering Where, 251–285. Hedley, N. R. (2001). Cognitive and Nocke, T., Flechsig, M., & Bohm, U. (2007). Usability Issues in Geovisualization. Visual exploration and evaluation of Cartography and Geographic Information climate-related simulation data. In Science, 28(1), 61–75. Simulation Conference, 2007 Winter (pp. https://doi.org/10.1559/152304001782173 703–711). IEEE. Retrieved from 998 http://ieeexplore.ieee.org/abstract/docume nt/4419664/