=Paper=
{{Paper
|id=Vol-2450/short3
|storemode=property
|title=The Effectiveness of Advice Solicitation and Social Peers in an Energy Recommender System
|pdfUrl=https://ceur-ws.org/Vol-2450/short3.pdf
|volume=Vol-2450
|authors=Alain Starke
|dblpUrl=https://dblp.org/rec/conf/recsys/Starke19
}}
==The Effectiveness of Advice Solicitation and Social Peers in an Energy Recommender System==
The effectiveness of advice solicitation and social peers in an energy recommender system Alain Starke Human-Technology Interaction Group Eindhoven University of Technology Eindhoven, the Netherlands A.D.Starke@tue.nl ABSTRACT own behavior: “this is what I would do”, even though this might not be as persuasive. In addition, whether one’s advice is In face-to-face interactions, advice acceptance depends on how it accepted might actually depend on who is giving advice: a good is presented, as well as a number of social factors. For example, friend or a stranger [2, 35]. some persons are inclined to accept advice from an expert if they Research has examined the effects of advice form, task possess little domain knowledge. In contrast, if such advice is difficulty, and advice source on whether this advice is accepted unsolicited, persons might only accept advice from a trusted by others [3, 6, 10]. Although each of these factors are important source, such as a family member. Whether these mechanisms on their own [6], their interplay might affect advice acceptance also play a role in the recommender context is unknown, even differently. For example, advice given by those who are similar though advice solicitation may be particularly important in or who are trusted, is more likely to be accepted than advice domains where a recommender user seeks behavioral change given by strangers [14]. However, if the task or decision at hand (e.g. energy conservation, healthy eating). This study examines is difficult, one might be more likely to trust and to accept expert the role of advice solicitation (i.e. whether one asks for advice or advice instead [9]. Nonetheless, this also depends on whether the simply receives it) and advice source (i.e. either explained in advice is asked for or unsolicited [12, 13]. social terms or not) in our ‘Saving Aid’ energy recommender Such mechanisms of advice-taking are given little attention in system. Through a web-based user study with 252 participants, an HCI context, but may impact whether a user actually acts on we find that allowing users to solicit advice themselves increases presented recommendations. Most recommender systems tend to their perceived level of trust with our energy recommender be prescriptive in presenting their content (“chosen for you”), system, compared to users that are presented unsolicited advice. and do not consider whether some users prefer to browse for In turn, we find that trust positively affects user satisfaction appropriate content instead. Moreover, although it has been levels, as well as the number of chosen energy-saving measures. shown that explaining recommendation in terms of social peers We discuss how system designers should consider how advice is increases the likelihood that items are chosen [4], as well as presented and in which context. evaluated satisfactorily [17, 28], these findings are usually limited to social network applications. CCS CONCEPTS The interplay between advice solicitation and advice source • Informa on Systems Decision Support Systems might be particularly important in domains where behavioral • Human‐centered Compu ng User Studies change is part of the recommender ecosystem, such as energy conservation and health [18, 25, 29, 33]. There, personalization is KEYWORDS merely the starting point of persuading a user to take up a new habit [7, 24, 31]. For example, besides predicting which specific Recommender Systems, Behavioral Change, Energy energy-saving measures are appropriate for a user, the use of Conserva on, User Experience, Advice Solicita on social comparison and peer feedback might ultimately persuade that user to change his or her energy conservation habits [23, 1 Introduction 27]. Moreover, there is evidence that users might only be willing When one gives another person advice, it is often worded in no to change their habits if they have solicited advice themselves, uncertain terms what the other person should do, for example by instead of the advice being ‘forced’ upon them [12]. saying “You should buy this item”. Such advice may be Furthermore, energy recommender systems need to consider unsolicited by the other person and, therefore, not accepted. In that each behavior has a different execution difficulty [25, 29]. other situations, much more precaution is taken by sharing one’s An effective energy recommender should persuade its users to choose and perform more energy-efficient behaviors [18, 29, 32]. IntRS ’19: Joint Workshop on Interfaces and Human Decision Making for Whether a user is willing to only make a small change in his Recommender Systems, September 19, 2019, Copenhagen, Denmark. conservation habits (e.g. changing one’s light bulbs), or a large Copyright © 2019 for this paper by its authors. Use permitted under Creative one (e.g. installing solar PV), may depend on which other users Commons License Attribution 4.0 International (CC BY 4.0). have already adopted a certain behavior [1, 23, 27]. IntRS ’19, September 19, 2019, Copenhagen, Denmark A.D. Starke 1.1 Research Question Furthermore, unsolicited advice also tends to be accepted when it is given by a friend or family member [10, 12]. However, This paper examines whether advice solicitation affects how a some users might feel as if the added social sources are ‘butting recommender system and its advice are evaluated. For example, in’, an experience that is often associated with unsolicited aid some users benefit from browsing a recommender’s personalized and which usually leads to poor rates of advice acceptance [3, 8]. interface [15], without requiring an explicit description of each Advice solicitation impacts advice acceptance, regardless of item’s fit. Moreover, whether users wish to solicit advice whether the advice is of high quality or not. According to Fisher themselves or that unsolicited advice is accepted, may depend on et al. [8], negative responses to aid (e.g. recommended items) the advice’s source. In this case, we examine whether the advice often stem from the recipient’s perceived threat to self-esteem or is explained in social terms or not, coming from either the autonomy. This is conceptualized as ‘threat to face’ [12]: positive system itself, a similar user, or an expert. face equals one’s positive self-image, whereas negative face We posit the following research question: relates to one’s autonomy and control over one’s own life [8]. [RQ]: To what extent do advice solicitation and social advice Threats to both types of face are lower when an advice recipient affect a user’s perception and evaluation of an energy (or user) seems to have asked for advice [12]. Threat to face recommender system? seems to be more important in conflict-avoiding cultures, such as In the upcoming section, we discuss which psychological those found in eastern Asia [21]. concepts underlie advice-giving and taking, and how advice In the context of an energy recommender system, the solicitation and social advice might influence these. In addition, addition of social sources might only work if the user has an to generate energy recommendations, we present a ‘light option to solicit advice or not, as it might otherwise pose a threat personalization’ algorithm using the psychometric Rasch model. to one’s autonomy and, in turn, decrease the trust and quality of In section 3, we present our ‘Saving Aid’ recommender system. the recommended advice. Hence, we expect that allowing users to solicit advice leads to different evaluations of a recommender 2 Related Work system in terms of trust and quality, compared to a system that presents unsolicited advice. Moreover, we expect this effect to 2.1 Advice form, acceptance, and autonomy depend on how the advice is explained, using either social peers When someone receives advice in a face-to-face situation, three or not. different motives on the advisee’s part are found to be important [3, 26]: increasing one’s decision accuracy, minimizing decision 2.2 Personalization in energy recommenders effort, and maintaining autonomy. Whereas the first two are Our research question is contextualized in the household energy typically addressed by recommender systems in the human- domain. Previous energy recommender studies have shown that computer interaction domain, the latter has received less a simple personalization algorithm, based on the psychometric attention in a personalization context. Rasch model, can lead to positive changes in user evaluations One’s autonomy in decision-making has traditionally been [29]. This item response theory model assumes that all energy- defined as one’s need to resist influence or coercion, or to strive saving measures and persons share a one-dimensional trait for for independence [22]. Although this begs the question why the goal of saving energy [34], which manifests itself as a single persons with a high need for autonomy would seek advice from measurement scale. others [20, 22], let alone recommender systems, much of the The Rasch model is an operationalization of ‘Campbell’s advice and personalization that one faces in life is unsolicited Paradigm’ [16]. This attitude theory postulates that one’s [12]. Even so, it is arguably inevitable that also a highly- energy-saving attitude becomes apparent through the behavioral autonomous person requires advice from others for complex steps that one is willing to take. Put differently, the more decisions where he or she lacks domain knowledge. measures one takes with the goal of saving energy, the stronger How advice is evaluated, appears to depend on the interplay one’s energy-saving attitude is expected to be [16, 29, 34]. In a between how advice representation and the advisee’s autonomy similar vein, these energy-saving measures are assumed to differ [5, 6]. For instance, prescriptive advice (‘you should do X’) is in how difficult they are to perform, which is operationalized as typically evaluated worse than descriptive advice (‘I would do their behavioral costs [16]. Measures that are performed less X’), if the person has a high need for autonomy [6]. However, if often are assumed to have higher behavioral costs, and vice the advice source has a high level of perceived expertise, versa [16, 34]. unsolicited and prescriptive advice tends to be evaluated more The Rasch model is described in Equation 1. A person n with favorably, compared to advice coming from a stranger [6, 13]. an attitude θn that exceeds the behavioral costs δi of an energy- This may be particularly important when a user lacks the saving measure i, has a high probability P of performing such capabilities to make an accurate decision [20], causing the user’s measure: P {Xni = 1}. In contrast, that probability is expected to be need for autonomy to diminish. For example, a recommender low if that person’s attitude is much lower than the behavioral system that presents items that are unfamiliar to a user, might costs of the measure at hand: want to explain its recommendations in terms of other expert users. 1 (1) Advice solicitation and social peers in energy recommender systems IntRS ’19, September 19, 2019, Copenhagen, Denmark Using the Rasch model, Starke et al. [29] have reliably fitted a savings (NL: ‘Besparing’) of each measure were listed. To one-dimensional scale of 79 energy-saving measures. These are personalize the Saving Aid, it first presented the best fitting either one-time investments (“insulate the exterior walls”), or measures according to the Rasch model (cf. Equation 1), frequent curtailment behaviors (“turn off lights after leaving a presenting those that were closest to a user’s attitude in terms of room”). The construct is used in the current study to present a their behavioral costs. Users were then asked to choose (NL: list of personalized energy-saving measures that strike the right ‘Kies’) any number of measures they would like to perform in balance between attractiveness (the behavioral costs are not too the weeks following the study. As soon as users had finished high for the user’s attitude) and novelty (the user is expected to navigating the interface and had chosen measures, they were not already perform each measure), by minimizing the difference presented a questionnaire on how they perceived the presented between a user’s energy-saving attitude and a measure’s recommendations, as well as how they evaluated the system. behavioral costs. If attitude and behavioral costs are equal, a measure’s engagement probability is 50% (cf. Equation 1). 3 Method To investigate how advice solicitation and social advice explanations affect a user’s evaluation of an energy recommender system, we performed a between-subjects web study. To do so, we developed the ‘Saving Aid’ recommender system (cf. Figure 2), a free-to-use ‘web shop’ that presented attitude-tailored energy-saving measures based on a user’s past behavior and the Rasch model. Users could choose any number of measures they wished to perform in the weeks following the study, which would be sent to them by email. 3.1 Procedure Figure 1 depicts the general procedure of the current recommender study, which was similar to the procedure used in an earlier study of Starke et al. [29]. First, we estimated each user’s energy-saving attitude, by surveying their current energy- saving behavior. Similar to other studies [30, 31], we used a short survey of thirteen energy-saving measures. To do so, we divided the Rasch scale in thirteen subsets across its entire behavioral Figure 2. Interface of the ‘Saving Aid’ energy cost range (from δ = −4.41 to δ = 4.42; Mδ = 0.05), and randomly recommender system (in Dutch). 79 measures were sampled a measure from each subset. For each measure, users presented in ascending order of behavioral costs (labeled had to indicate whether they already performed it (‘yes’ or ‘no’), as ranging from ‘easy/popular’ on the left, to or that a measure did not apply to their housing situation (e.g. ‘challenging/less popular’ on the right), and could be energy-efficient garden lighting did not apply to a user who did freely navigated using the colored blocks. Each measure listed its name (e.g. “Install double-glazed windows” on the not own a garden). left), as well as its investment costs (NL: “Kosten”) and The estimated attitude θ was based on the number of ‘yes’ projected kWh Savings (NL: “Besparing”). The text at the responses. We used the average behavioral cost level δ of the top: “klik hier […] u aanraden,” translates to “click here if equivalent Rasch scale subset. For example, if a user had you would like to see what others recommend you”. submitted six ‘yes’ responses, the attitude was estimated to be equal to the average behavioral costs of the sixth subset. 3.2 Research Design How the advice and the recommender interface were presented, was subject to a 3x2 between-subjects research design. For each condition, we changed how advice was presented. On the one hand, we discerned between three different sources of advice: pointing out that specific measures were recommended by either Figure 1. Procedure of the current recommender study. an expert, similar users, or the system itself. These would be added as explanations to the system’s solicitation button (cf. top Subsequently, users were navigated to the ‘Saving Aid’ of Figure 2) or the unsolicited measures (cf. Figure 3). interface, which is shown in Figure 2. It presented the Rasch On the other hand, we differentiated between two levels of scale of 79 energy-saving measures in ascending order of advice solicitation. In the unsolicited advice condition, users behavioral costs (from ‘popular’ to ‘challenging’), which users were immediately presented three different energy-saving were free to navigate. The name, costs (NL: ‘Kosten’) and kWh measures in the pop-up screen depicted in Figure 3. They were IntRS ’19, September 19, 2019, Copenhagen, Denmark A.D. Starke free to choose any of these measures, or could continue to the 3.4 Measures main interface. In the solicited advice condition, users were only shown a pop-up that explained how the interface worked, after 3.4.1. Objective aspects which they could continue to the main Saving Aid interface. The different recommender conditions (presenting either However, users could solicit personalized recommendations in solicited or unsolicited advice, from either a similar source, an the main interface by clicking a button, which is depicted at the expert, or the system itself) were considered as objective changes top of Figure 2: ‘click here if you want to see what others would in the interface. We tested whether these changes affected how recommend to you’. users perceived the presented recommendations and, in turn, evaluated the system. Furthermore, we also examined whether this led to changes in the number of chosen measures. Upon analysis, we observed that only half of all users in the ‘solicited’ condition had actually clicked to solicit personalized recommendations (cf. Figure 2). Since we expected that either inspecting a recommendation list or not could affect how users perceived and evaluated the system, we decided to discern between three groups of solicitation instead: unsolicited advice (i.e. baseline), no advice solicitation (did not click), and advice solicitation. Since testing a 3x3 design with a sample of 252 participants would only allow us to detect rather large effects, we collapsed the three social conditions (system, similar, or expert) into two: advice from either a social or non-social source. This allowed us to interpret the results with sufficient statistical power. Note that we found no significant differences between similar and expert advice in a separate analysis. Figure 3. The pop-up screen in the unsolicited condition, 3.4.2. Subjective aspects which presented energy-saving advice immediately after We surveyed users on five subjective constructs: perceived attitude calibration. Similar to Figure 2, each measure’s recommendation quality, perceived system trust, threat to name, costs, and kWh savings was listed. This example negative face (i.e. whether one’s autonomy is affected), system depicts advice explained through an expert source (NL: satisfaction, and choice satisfaction. For all these aspects, users “expert advies”). Cropped off the top of the image is an explanatory text in Dutch. Users could choose (NL: “Kies”) were presented survey items on 7-point Likert scales and were any of these measures if they wished to perform them. asked to indicate to what extent they agreed with each item. The used questionnaire items are described in Table 1. As prescribed by Knijnenburg and Willemsen [19], we 3.3 Participants submitted all responses to a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) In total, 260 participants used our Saving Aid recommender using ordinal dependent variables. Table 1 reports only three system and finished the subsequent questionnaire. Among them, user experience constructs, as we could not include the threat to 110 participants were recruited from the Jan Frederik Schouten face construct in our analysis, for it had high cross loadings with database of Eindhoven University of Technology, while others all other aspects in our model. Moreover, we could not discern were recruited through posts on social media (e.g. Twitter, choice satisfaction from the system satisfaction construct, since Facebook, etc.) Each participant entered a raffle in which they it violated divergent validity [19]. The remaining constructs met had a 20% probability to win €15. We omitted 8 participants from the guidelines for convergence validity, as the average variance analysis, as they either had finished the study in no more than 2 explained (AVE) for each construct was higher than 0.5, and had minutes, or showed no variation in their answers on the a good internal consistency (0.8 < α < 0.9) [11]. evaluation questionnaire. Eventually, we analyzed a sample of 252 participants (50.8% 4 Results female), which had a mean age of 31.9 years (SD = 16.0), and who on average chose 8.9 measures (SD = 9.1). Furthermore, We used Structural Equation Modeling (SEM) to organize all approximately half of the sample was still a university student, objective and subjective constructs, including relevant who typically lived in shared apartments. Most of these students interactions, into a path model. As prescribed by Knijnenburg had an income that fell below €1000. and Willemsen [19], a confirmatory factor analysis was performed first (cf. Table 1), after which we tested a fully saturated model and performed stepwise removal of non- significant relations. Figure 4 depicts the final path model, which had an excellent fit: χ2(113) = 131.625, p = 0.11, CFI = 0.995, TLI = 0.996, RMSEA = 0.026, 90%-CI: [0.000,0.042]. Advice solicitation and social peers in energy recommender systems IntRS ’19, September 19, 2019, Copenhagen, Denmark Table 1. Results of the confirmatory factory analysis on satisfaction showed that this effect was significant, mediated by user experience. Items without loading were removed perceived trust: β = .151, 95%-CI: [0.004,0.298], p = .044. from the final model, while the Choice Satisfaction and Second, Figure 4 also shows two different effects of users who Threat to Negative Face aspects were excluded as they were in the solicited condition, but who did not solicit advice. If violated divergent validity. The average variance such advice was offered using a socially-laden explanation explained (AVE) and Cronbach’s Alpha of other aspects (either through expert advice or similar peers), it negatively met the prescribed guidelines [11, 19]. impacted the perceived recommendation quality compared to the unsolicited, non-social baseline: β = −1.18, p < 0.01. In contrast, Aspect Item Loa‐ non-social explanations positively affected the perceived ding recommendation quality compared to the unsolicited baseline: β Choice I am happy with the measures I’ve chosen Satisfaction I know several measures that are better than = .41, p < 0.05. Since non-soliciting users did not actually see a the ones I selected list of highlighted energy-saving recommendations, it was I would recommend some of the chosen possible they evaluated the main interface instead, which measures to others confounds a clear interpretation of this result. I am looking forward to implement the chosen measures The measures I’ve chosen fit me seamlessly Perceived I found the recommended measures to be .813 Rec. Quality attractive The recommended measures fitted my .857 AVE: .68 preferences Alpha: .82 The recommended measures were relevant to .731 me The Saving Aid proposed too many bad measures I did not like any of the measures Perceived I think that the Saving Aid was telling me the .756 Sys. Trust truth I expected the Saving Aid to be truthful .670 AVE: .72 The Saving Aid was honest .747 Alpha: .88 The Saving Aid was sufficiently Figure 4. Structural Equation Model (SEM). The numbers knowledgeable to present advice on the arrows represent β-coefficients, standard errors are The Saving Aid had the best intentions .624 denoted between brackets. The effects between the latent subjective constructs are standardized and can be System The Saving Aid has made me more aware of .546 considered as correlations. Aspects are grouped by color: satisfaction my energy-saving behavior objective system aspects are purple, interaction aspects are I would like to use the Saving Aid more often .663 blue, subjective aspects are green, and experience aspects AVE: .70 I make better decisions using the Saving Aid .710 are orange. *** p < 0.001, ** p < 0.01, * p < 0.05. Alpha: .88 The Saving Aid helps me to find appropriate .620 measures The Saving Aid allowed me to choose 4.2 System evaluation and choice behavior measures easily Figure 4 depicts that users who had chosen a measure from Threat to The Saving Aid respected my autonomy and either a solicited or unsolicited recommendation list, perceived Negative the choices I made the recommendation quality to be higher than those who did Face The Saving Aid did not impose anything on not: β = .636, p < 0.001. Furthermore, Figure 4 shows two me I was free to choose any measure different pathways in which higher levels of recommendation I did not feel forced to take the Saving Aid’s quality increase other subjective aspects. First, higher perceived advice recommendation quality positively affected perceived trust and, in turn, system satisfaction. Besides this mediated effect, Figure 4 4.1 Advice solicitation also shows a positive, direct path from recommendation quality Figure 4 shows two effects of advice solicitation on how the to system satisfaction, as well as a positive effect from users recommender system and its advice were perceived. First, users choosing a recommended measure to system satisfaction. who had the option to solicit advice and actually did so (‘advice Moreover, a positive evaluation of the system also led users to solicitation’), reported higher levels of system trust than those choose more energy-saving measures: β = .979, p < 0.05. who were presented unsolicited advice: β = .414, p < 0.05. This Although it could be possible to reverse this particular causal suggested that users who asked for personalized advice direction (e.g. more choices led to higher satisfaction levels), the perceived the system as more trustworthy, than those who current pathway was consistent with previous energy immediately faced unsolicited advice, regardless of the social recommender studies [18, 29], and led to the best model fit source. A bootstrapped test of indirect effects towards system statistics. IntRS ’19, September 19, 2019, Copenhagen, Denmark A.D. Starke 5 Discussion 5.1 Limitations As one of the first to do so, this study has applied the Unfortunately, we could not relate our path model constructs to phenomenon of face-to-face advice solicitation (asking for advice choice satisfaction, for it violated divergent validity. However, or not) to the HCI domain. We have investigated to what extent the strong correlation between both system and choice solicitation and social explanations of advice affect how a user satisfaction suggests that trust would also have a positive effect perceives and evaluates an energy recommender system. on how users evaluate their choices, which has shown to To do so, we have developed the ‘Saving Aid’ system, from positively affect the probability that chosen measures are which users could choose any number of energy-saving actually implemented and a user’s behavior is changed [29]. measures they would like to perform. It employs a simple Our findings are confounded by not all users soliciting advice personalization algorithm, using the psychometric Rasch model, in the ‘solicitation condition’. This has prevented us from to generate appropriate attitude-tailored recommendations for analyzing our original research design, which has now each user. In this personalized advice context, we have found overlooked any differences between specific social explanations, small differences in how our energy recommender system is either a similar peer or an expert. Although this should be evaluated, based on whether advice is solicited by users or further investigated in a follow-up study, the discrepancy presented without being explicitly solicited. between social and non-social explanations of advice seems to be Our results show that users who have solicited personalized an important finding in itself, which is also demonstrated by the recommendations report higher system trust levels, compared to interaction effect in our path model. those who are presented unsolicited advice. It could be that users Furthermore, in studies on face-to-face interactions, advice wish to maintain control over which items they inspect, and that solicitation is typically related to personality concepts as a system that immediately determines which three items are autonomy and threat to face. Due to a violation of divergent appropriate, is perceived as less trustworthy. The fact that most validity in our path model, we have been unable to test whether recommender research does not consider whether some users a violation of autonomy has decreased trust. Nonetheless, our wish to solicit advice themselves, seems to be a missed study still shows that changes in the recommender interface, due opportunity, since higher trust levels in a recommender could to advice solicitation and social explanations, lead to changes in have important second-order effects. Indeed, in this study, we system perception and choice behavior, and should be find that users who report higher trust levels, also choose more considered in future recommender designs. energy-saving measures as a result of a positive user experience. Furthermore, we find mixed results for perceived ACKNOWLEDGMENTS recommendation quality across social conditions. Users who We thank Nena van As, Jochem Bek, and Boy Janissen for their have the opportunity to solicit advice but do not do so, report time and effort to conduct this study and discuss its implications. lower levels of recommendation quality if advice is explained in This work is part of the Research Talent program with project terms of similar peers or experts, compared to users who face number 406-14-088, which is financed by the Netherlands unsolicited social advice. This effect reverses for non-social Organization for Scientific Research (NWO). It was also made explanations, as users who are presented the opportunity to possible by funding from the Niels Stensen Fellowship. solicit system advice report higher recommendation quality than those who are presented unsolicited system advice. These REFERENCES findings either suggest that adding social explanations to [1] Allcott, H. 2011. Social norms and energy conservation. Journal of Public unsolicited advice could mitigate a user’s feeling that the system Economics. 95, 9–10 (Oct. 2011), 1082–1095. is ‘butting in’, or that users who could solicit advice might not be DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpubeco.2011.03.003. [2] Bo Feng and MacGeorge, E.L. 2010. The Influences of Message and Source interested in additional social advice after inspecting the main Factors on Advice Outcomes. Communication Research. 37, 4 (Aug. 2010), Saving Aid interface. However, these comparisons only apply to 553–575. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650210368258. [3] Bonaccio, S. and Dalal, R.S. 2006. Advice taking and decision-making: An those who have not clicked, which has probably increased the integrative literature review, and implications for the organizational differences between conditions. sciences. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 101, 2 (Nov. 2006), 127–151. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2006.07.001. Our results resonate with earlier findings on inspectability [4] Bonhard, P. and Sasse, M.A. 2006. ’Knowing me, knowing you’ — Using and control in social recommender systems [17], which has profiles and social networking to improve recommender systems. BT shown to positively affect system evaluation. Furthermore, it Technology Journal. 24, 3 (Jul. 2006), 84–98. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10550-006-0080-3. shows that findings from the advice-giving and taking literature [5] Caplan, S.E. and Samter, W. 1999. The role of facework in younger and on face-to-face interactions do translate to the HCI context [e.g. older adults’ evaluations of social support messages. Communication Quarterly. 47, 3 (1999), 245–264. 3, 12]. Even though the interactions in a typical recommender [6] Dalal, R.S. and Bonaccio, S. 2010. What types of advice do decision-makers system are hardly anthropomorphic, the principles of trust, prefer? Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 112, 1 (May quality, and possibly autonomy might also translate to 2010), 11–23. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2009.11.007. [7] Ekstrand, M.D. and Willemsen, M.C. 2016. Behaviorism is Not Enough: personalized HCI. These factors may be particularly important in Better Recommendations Through Listening to Users. Proceedings of the domains where users wish to change their lifestyle, such as 10th ACM Conference on Recommender Systems (New York, NY, USA, 2016), 221–224. health and energy conservation, which often rely on social proof [8] Fisher, J.D., Nadler, A. and Whitcher-Alagna, S. 1982. Recipient reactions to achieve behavioral change [1, 27]. to aid. Psychological Bulletin. 91, 1 (1982), 27. Advice solicitation and social peers in energy recommender systems IntRS ’19, September 19, 2019, Copenhagen, Denmark [9] Gino, F. and Moore, D.A. 2007. Effects of task difficulty on use of advice. [33] Trattner, C. and Elsweiler, D. 2017. Food Recommender Systems: Journal of Behavioral Decision Making. 20, 1 (Jan. 2007), 21–35. Important Contributions, Challenges and Future Research Directions. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.539. arXiv:1711.02760 [cs]. (Nov. 2017). [10] Gino, F., Shang, J. and Croson, R. 2009. The impact of information from [34] Urban, J. and Ščasný, M. 2016. Structure of Domestic Energy Saving: How similar or different advisors on judgment. Organizational Behavior and Many Dimensions? Environment and Behavior. 48, 3 (Apr. 2016), 454–481. Human Decision Processes. 108, 2 (Mar. 2009), 287–302. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916514547081. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2008.08.002. [35] Yaniv, I., Choshen-Hillel, S. and Milyavsky, M. 2011. Receiving advice on [11] Gliem, J.A. and Gliem, R.R. 2003. Calculating, Interpreting, And Reporting matters of taste: Similarity, majority influence, and taste discrimination. Cronbach’s Alpha Reliability Coefficient For Likert-Type Scales. (2003). Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 115, 1 (May 2011), [12] Goldsmith, D.J. 2000. Soliciting advice: The role of sequential placement in 111–120. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2010.11.006. mitigating face threat. Communication Monographs. 67, 1 (Mar. 2000), 1–19. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1080/03637750009376492. [13] Goldsmith, D.J. and Fitch, K. 1997. The Normative Context of Advice as Social Support. Human Communication Research. 23, 4 (Jun. 1997), 454–476. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.1997.tb00406.x. [14] Harvey, N. and Fischer, I. 1997. Taking Advice: Accepting Help, Improving Judgment, and Sharing Responsibility. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 70, 2 (May 1997), 117–133. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1006/obhd.1997.2697. [15] Herlocker, J.L., Konstan, J.A., Terveen, L.G. and Riedl, J.T. 2004. Evaluating Collaborative Filtering Recommender Systems. ACM Trans. Inf. Syst. 22, 1 (Jan. 2004), 5–53. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1145/963770.963772. [16] Kaiser, F.G., Byrka, K. and Hartig, T. 2010. Reviving Campbell’s Paradigm for Attitude Research. Personality and Social Psychology Review. 14, 4 (Nov. 2010), 351–367. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868310366452. [17] Knijnenburg, B.P., Bostandjiev, S., O’Donovan, J. and Kobsa, A. 2012. Inspectability and control in social recommenders. Proceedings of the sixth ACM conference on Recommender systems (2012), 43–50. [18] Knijnenburg, B.P., Willemsen, M. and Broeders, R. 2014. Smart sustainability through system satisfaction: Tailored preference elicitation for energy-saving recommenders. Proceedings 20th Americas Conference on Information Systems (AMCIS 2014): Smart Sustainability: The Information Systems Opportunity (Savannah, Georgia, United States, 2014). [19] Knijnenburg, B.P. and Willemsen, M.C. 2015. Evaluating recommender systems with user experiments. Recommender Systems Handbook. Springer. 309–352. [20] Koestner, R., Gingras, I., Abutaa, R., Losier, G.F., DiDio, L. and Gagné, M. 1999. To Follow Expert Advice When Making a Decision: An Examination of Reactive Versus Reflective Autonomy. Journal of Personality. 67, 5 (1999), 851–872. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-6494.00075. [21] Meyer, E. 2014. The culture map: Breaking through the invisible boundaries of global business. Public Affairs. [22] Murray, H.A. 1938. Explorations in personality: A clinical and experimental study of fifty men of college age. (1938). [23] Nolan, J.M., Schultz, P.W., Cialdini, R.B., Goldstein, N.J. and Griskevicius, V. 2008. Normative social influence is underdetected. Personality and social psychology bulletin. 34, 7 (2008), 913–923. [24] Schäfer, H., Hors-Fraile, S., Karumur, R.P., Calero Valdez, A., Said, A., Torkamaan, H., Ulmer, T. and Trattner, C. 2017. Towards Health (Aware) Recommender Systems. Proceedings of the 2017 International Conference on Digital Health (New York, NY, USA, 2017), 157–161. [25] Schäfer, H. and Willemsen, M.C. 2019. Rasch-based Tailored Goals for Nutrition Assistance Systems. Proceedings of the 24th International Conference on Intelligent User Interfaces (New York, NY, USA, 2019), 18–29. [26] Schrah, G.E., Dalal, R.S. and Sniezek, J.A. 2006. No decision-maker is an Island: integrating expert advice with information acquisition. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making. 19, 1 (Jan. 2006), 43–60. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.514. [27] Schultz, P.W., Nolan, J.M., Cialdini, R.B., Goldstein, N.J. and Griskevicius, V. 2007. The constructive, destructive, and reconstructive power of social norms. Psychological science. 18, 5 (2007), 429–434. [28] Sharma, A. and Cosley, D. 2013. Do social explanations work?: studying and modeling the effects of social explanations in recommender systems. Proceedings of the 22nd international conference on World Wide Web (2013), 1133–1144. [29] Starke, A., Willemsen, M. and Snijders, C. 2017. Effective User Interface Designs to Increase Energy-efficient Behavior in a Rasch-based Energy Recommender System. Proceedings of the Eleventh ACM Conference on Recommender Systems (New York, NY, USA, 2017), 65–73. [30] Starke, A., Willemsen, M.C. and Snijders, C. 2015. Saving Energy in 1-D: Tailoring Energy-saving Advice Using a Rasch-based Energy Recommender System. DMRS (2015), 5–8. [31] Starke, A.D. 2019. Supporting energy-efficient choices using Rasch-based recommender interfaces. (2019). [32] Tomkins, S., Isley, S. and Getoor, L. 2018. Sustainability at Scale: Towards Bridging the Intention-Behavior Gap with Sustainable Recommendations. Proceedings of the 12th ACM Conference on Recommender Systems (Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada, 2018), 214–218.