=Paper= {{Paper |id=Vol-2450/short3 |storemode=property |title=The Effectiveness of Advice Solicitation and Social Peers in an Energy Recommender System |pdfUrl=https://ceur-ws.org/Vol-2450/short3.pdf |volume=Vol-2450 |authors=Alain Starke |dblpUrl=https://dblp.org/rec/conf/recsys/Starke19 }} ==The Effectiveness of Advice Solicitation and Social Peers in an Energy Recommender System== https://ceur-ws.org/Vol-2450/short3.pdf
   The effectiveness of advice solicitation and social peers in an
                  energy recommender system
                                                                               Alain Starke
                                                            Human-Technology Interaction Group
                                                            Eindhoven University of Technology
                                                                Eindhoven, the Netherlands
                                                                    A.D.Starke@tue.nl


ABSTRACT                                                                               own behavior: “this is what I would do”, even though this might
                                                                                       not be as persuasive. In addition, whether one’s advice is
In face-to-face interactions, advice acceptance depends on how it                      accepted might actually depend on who is giving advice: a good
is presented, as well as a number of social factors. For example,                      friend or a stranger [2, 35].
some persons are inclined to accept advice from an expert if they                          Research has examined the effects of advice form, task
possess little domain knowledge. In contrast, if such advice is                        difficulty, and advice source on whether this advice is accepted
unsolicited, persons might only accept advice from a trusted                           by others [3, 6, 10]. Although each of these factors are important
source, such as a family member. Whether these mechanisms                              on their own [6], their interplay might affect advice acceptance
also play a role in the recommender context is unknown, even                           differently. For example, advice given by those who are similar
though advice solicitation may be particularly important in                            or who are trusted, is more likely to be accepted than advice
domains where a recommender user seeks behavioral change                               given by strangers [14]. However, if the task or decision at hand
(e.g. energy conservation, healthy eating). This study examines                        is difficult, one might be more likely to trust and to accept expert
the role of advice solicitation (i.e. whether one asks for advice or                   advice instead [9]. Nonetheless, this also depends on whether the
simply receives it) and advice source (i.e. either explained in                        advice is asked for or unsolicited [12, 13].
social terms or not) in our ‘Saving Aid’ energy recommender                                Such mechanisms of advice-taking are given little attention in
system. Through a web-based user study with 252 participants,                          an HCI context, but may impact whether a user actually acts on
we find that allowing users to solicit advice themselves increases                     presented recommendations. Most recommender systems tend to
their perceived level of trust with our energy recommender                             be prescriptive in presenting their content (“chosen for you”),
system, compared to users that are presented unsolicited advice.                       and do not consider whether some users prefer to browse for
In turn, we find that trust positively affects user satisfaction                       appropriate content instead. Moreover, although it has been
levels, as well as the number of chosen energy-saving measures.                        shown that explaining recommendation in terms of social peers
We discuss how system designers should consider how advice is                          increases the likelihood that items are chosen [4], as well as
presented and in which context.                                                        evaluated satisfactorily [17, 28], these findings are usually
                                                                                       limited to social network applications.
CCS CONCEPTS                                                                               The interplay between advice solicitation and advice source
• Informa on Systems  Decision Support Systems                                        might be particularly important in domains where behavioral
• Human‐centered Compu ng  User Studies                                               change is part of the recommender ecosystem, such as energy
                                                                                       conservation and health [18, 25, 29, 33]. There, personalization is
KEYWORDS                                                                               merely the starting point of persuading a user to take up a new
                                                                                       habit [7, 24, 31]. For example, besides predicting which specific
Recommender Systems, Behavioral Change, Energy                                         energy-saving measures are appropriate for a user, the use of
Conserva on, User Experience, Advice Solicita on                                       social comparison and peer feedback might ultimately persuade
                                                                                       that user to change his or her energy conservation habits [23,
      1     Introduction                                                               27]. Moreover, there is evidence that users might only be willing
When one gives another person advice, it is often worded in no                         to change their habits if they have solicited advice themselves,
uncertain terms what the other person should do, for example by                        instead of the advice being ‘forced’ upon them [12].
saying “You should buy this item”. Such advice may be                                      Furthermore, energy recommender systems need to consider
unsolicited by the other person and, therefore, not accepted. In                       that each behavior has a different execution difficulty [25, 29].
other situations, much more precaution is taken by sharing one’s                       An effective energy recommender should persuade its users to
                                                                                       choose and perform more energy-efficient behaviors [18, 29, 32].
IntRS ’19: Joint Workshop on Interfaces and Human Decision Making for                  Whether a user is willing to only make a small change in his
Recommender Systems, September 19, 2019, Copenhagen, Denmark.                          conservation habits (e.g. changing one’s light bulbs), or a large
Copyright © 2019 for this paper by its authors. Use permitted under Creative
                                                                                       one (e.g. installing solar PV), may depend on which other users
Commons License Attribution 4.0 International (CC BY 4.0).
                                                                                       have already adopted a certain behavior [1, 23, 27].
IntRS ’19, September 19, 2019, Copenhagen, Denmark                                                                              A.D. Starke


1.1 Research Question                                                     Furthermore, unsolicited advice also tends to be accepted
                                                                      when it is given by a friend or family member [10, 12]. However,
This paper examines whether advice solicitation affects how a
                                                                      some users might feel as if the added social sources are ‘butting
recommender system and its advice are evaluated. For example,
                                                                      in’, an experience that is often associated with unsolicited aid
some users benefit from browsing a recommender’s personalized
                                                                      and which usually leads to poor rates of advice acceptance [3, 8].
interface [15], without requiring an explicit description of each
                                                                          Advice solicitation impacts advice acceptance, regardless of
item’s fit. Moreover, whether users wish to solicit advice
                                                                      whether the advice is of high quality or not. According to Fisher
themselves or that unsolicited advice is accepted, may depend on
                                                                      et al. [8], negative responses to aid (e.g. recommended items)
the advice’s source. In this case, we examine whether the advice
                                                                      often stem from the recipient’s perceived threat to self-esteem or
is explained in social terms or not, coming from either the
                                                                      autonomy. This is conceptualized as ‘threat to face’ [12]: positive
system itself, a similar user, or an expert.
                                                                      face equals one’s positive self-image, whereas negative face
   We posit the following research question:
                                                                      relates to one’s autonomy and control over one’s own life [8].
   [RQ]: To what extent do advice solicitation and social advice
                                                                      Threats to both types of face are lower when an advice recipient
affect a user’s perception and evaluation of an energy
                                                                      (or user) seems to have asked for advice [12]. Threat to face
recommender system?
                                                                      seems to be more important in conflict-avoiding cultures, such as
   In the upcoming section, we discuss which psychological
                                                                      those found in eastern Asia [21].
concepts underlie advice-giving and taking, and how advice
                                                                          In the context of an energy recommender system, the
solicitation and social advice might influence these. In addition,
                                                                      addition of social sources might only work if the user has an
to generate energy recommendations, we present a ‘light
                                                                      option to solicit advice or not, as it might otherwise pose a threat
personalization’ algorithm using the psychometric Rasch model.
                                                                      to one’s autonomy and, in turn, decrease the trust and quality of
In section 3, we present our ‘Saving Aid’ recommender system.
                                                                      the recommended advice. Hence, we expect that allowing users
                                                                      to solicit advice leads to different evaluations of a recommender
     2    Related Work                                                system in terms of trust and quality, compared to a system that
                                                                      presents unsolicited advice. Moreover, we expect this effect to
2.1 Advice form, acceptance, and autonomy
                                                                      depend on how the advice is explained, using either social peers
When someone receives advice in a face-to-face situation, three       or not.
different motives on the advisee’s part are found to be important
[3, 26]: increasing one’s decision accuracy, minimizing decision      2.2 Personalization in energy recommenders
effort, and maintaining autonomy. Whereas the first two are
                                                                      Our research question is contextualized in the household energy
typically addressed by recommender systems in the human-
                                                                      domain. Previous energy recommender studies have shown that
computer interaction domain, the latter has received less
                                                                      a simple personalization algorithm, based on the psychometric
attention in a personalization context.
                                                                      Rasch model, can lead to positive changes in user evaluations
    One’s autonomy in decision-making has traditionally been
                                                                      [29]. This item response theory model assumes that all energy-
defined as one’s need to resist influence or coercion, or to strive
                                                                      saving measures and persons share a one-dimensional trait for
for independence [22]. Although this begs the question why
                                                                      the goal of saving energy [34], which manifests itself as a single
persons with a high need for autonomy would seek advice from
                                                                      measurement scale.
others [20, 22], let alone recommender systems, much of the
                                                                         The Rasch model is an operationalization of ‘Campbell’s
advice and personalization that one faces in life is unsolicited
                                                                      Paradigm’ [16]. This attitude theory postulates that one’s
[12]. Even so, it is arguably inevitable that also a highly-
                                                                      energy-saving attitude becomes apparent through the behavioral
autonomous person requires advice from others for complex
                                                                      steps that one is willing to take. Put differently, the more
decisions where he or she lacks domain knowledge.
                                                                      measures one takes with the goal of saving energy, the stronger
    How advice is evaluated, appears to depend on the interplay
                                                                      one’s energy-saving attitude is expected to be [16, 29, 34]. In a
between how advice representation and the advisee’s autonomy
                                                                      similar vein, these energy-saving measures are assumed to differ
[5, 6]. For instance, prescriptive advice (‘you should do X’) is
                                                                      in how difficult they are to perform, which is operationalized as
typically evaluated worse than descriptive advice (‘I would do
                                                                      their behavioral costs [16]. Measures that are performed less
X’), if the person has a high need for autonomy [6]. However, if
                                                                      often are assumed to have higher behavioral costs, and vice
the advice source has a high level of perceived expertise,
                                                                      versa [16, 34].
unsolicited and prescriptive advice tends to be evaluated more
                                                                         The Rasch model is described in Equation 1. A person n with
favorably, compared to advice coming from a stranger [6, 13].
                                                                      an attitude θn that exceeds the behavioral costs δi of an energy-
This may be particularly important when a user lacks the
                                                                      saving measure i, has a high probability P of performing such
capabilities to make an accurate decision [20], causing the user’s
                                                                      measure: P {Xni = 1}. In contrast, that probability is expected to be
need for autonomy to diminish. For example, a recommender
                                                                      low if that person’s attitude is much lower than the behavioral
system that presents items that are unfamiliar to a user, might
                                                                      costs of the measure at hand:
want to explain its recommendations in terms of other expert
users.                                                                                             1                               (1)
Advice solicitation and social peers in energy recommender systems                      IntRS ’19, September 19, 2019, Copenhagen, Denmark

    Using the Rasch model, Starke et al. [29] have reliably fitted a   savings (NL: ‘Besparing’) of each measure were listed. To
one-dimensional scale of 79 energy-saving measures. These are          personalize the Saving Aid, it first presented the best fitting
either one-time investments (“insulate the exterior walls”), or        measures according to the Rasch model (cf. Equation 1),
frequent curtailment behaviors (“turn off lights after leaving a       presenting those that were closest to a user’s attitude in terms of
room”). The construct is used in the current study to present a        their behavioral costs. Users were then asked to choose (NL:
list of personalized energy-saving measures that strike the right      ‘Kies’) any number of measures they would like to perform in
balance between attractiveness (the behavioral costs are not too       the weeks following the study. As soon as users had finished
high for the user’s attitude) and novelty (the user is expected to     navigating the interface and had chosen measures, they were
not already perform each measure), by minimizing the difference        presented a questionnaire on how they perceived the presented
between a user’s energy-saving attitude and a measure’s                recommendations, as well as how they evaluated the system.
behavioral costs. If attitude and behavioral costs are equal, a
measure’s engagement probability is 50% (cf. Equation 1).

      3 Method
To investigate how advice solicitation and social advice
explanations affect a user’s evaluation of an energy
recommender system, we performed a between-subjects web
study. To do so, we developed the ‘Saving Aid’ recommender
system (cf. Figure 2), a free-to-use ‘web shop’ that presented
attitude-tailored energy-saving measures based on a user’s past
behavior and the Rasch model. Users could choose any number
of measures they wished to perform in the weeks following the
study, which would be sent to them by email.

3.1 Procedure
Figure 1 depicts the general procedure of the current
recommender study, which was similar to the procedure used in
an earlier study of Starke et al. [29]. First, we estimated each
user’s energy-saving attitude, by surveying their current energy-
saving behavior. Similar to other studies [30, 31], we used a short
survey of thirteen energy-saving measures. To do so, we divided
the Rasch scale in thirteen subsets across its entire behavioral       Figure 2. Interface of the ‘Saving Aid’ energy
cost range (from δ = −4.41 to δ = 4.42; Mδ = 0.05), and randomly       recommender system (in Dutch). 79 measures were
sampled a measure from each subset. For each measure, users            presented in ascending order of behavioral costs (labeled
had to indicate whether they already performed it (‘yes’ or ‘no’),     as ranging from ‘easy/popular’ on the left, to
or that a measure did not apply to their housing situation (e.g.       ‘challenging/less popular’ on the right), and could be
energy-efficient garden lighting did not apply to a user who did       freely navigated using the colored blocks. Each measure
                                                                       listed its name (e.g. “Install double-glazed windows” on the
not own a garden).
                                                                       left), as well as its investment costs (NL: “Kosten”) and
   The estimated attitude θ was based on the number of ‘yes’
                                                                       projected kWh Savings (NL: “Besparing”). The text at the
responses. We used the average behavioral cost level δ of the          top: “klik hier […] u aanraden,” translates to “click here if
equivalent Rasch scale subset. For example, if a user had              you would like to see what others recommend you”.
submitted six ‘yes’ responses, the attitude was estimated to be
equal to the average behavioral costs of the sixth subset.             3.2 Research Design
                                                                       How the advice and the recommender interface were presented,
                                                                       was subject to a 3x2 between-subjects research design. For each
                                                                       condition, we changed how advice was presented. On the one
                                                                       hand, we discerned between three different sources of advice:
                                                                       pointing out that specific measures were recommended by either
Figure 1. Procedure of the current recommender study.                  an expert, similar users, or the system itself. These would be
                                                                       added as explanations to the system’s solicitation button (cf. top
   Subsequently, users were navigated to the ‘Saving Aid’              of Figure 2) or the unsolicited measures (cf. Figure 3).
interface, which is shown in Figure 2. It presented the Rasch             On the other hand, we differentiated between two levels of
scale of 79 energy-saving measures in ascending order of               advice solicitation. In the unsolicited advice condition, users
behavioral costs (from ‘popular’ to ‘challenging’), which users        were immediately presented three different energy-saving
were free to navigate. The name, costs (NL: ‘Kosten’) and kWh          measures in the pop-up screen depicted in Figure 3. They were
IntRS ’19, September 19, 2019, Copenhagen, Denmark                                                                             A.D. Starke

free to choose any of these measures, or could continue to the       3.4 Measures
main interface. In the solicited advice condition, users were only
shown a pop-up that explained how the interface worked, after        3.4.1. Objective aspects
which they could continue to the main Saving Aid interface.          The different recommender conditions (presenting either
However, users could solicit personalized recommendations in         solicited or unsolicited advice, from either a similar source, an
the main interface by clicking a button, which is depicted at the    expert, or the system itself) were considered as objective changes
top of Figure 2: ‘click here if you want to see what others would    in the interface. We tested whether these changes affected how
recommend to you’.                                                   users perceived the presented recommendations and, in turn,
                                                                     evaluated the system. Furthermore, we also examined whether
                                                                     this led to changes in the number of chosen measures.
                                                                         Upon analysis, we observed that only half of all users in the
                                                                     ‘solicited’ condition had actually clicked to solicit personalized
                                                                     recommendations (cf. Figure 2). Since we expected that either
                                                                     inspecting a recommendation list or not could affect how users
                                                                     perceived and evaluated the system, we decided to discern
                                                                     between three groups of solicitation instead: unsolicited advice
                                                                     (i.e. baseline), no advice solicitation (did not click), and advice
                                                                     solicitation. Since testing a 3x3 design with a sample of 252
                                                                     participants would only allow us to detect rather large effects,
                                                                     we collapsed the three social conditions (system, similar, or
                                                                     expert) into two: advice from either a social or non-social source.
                                                                     This allowed us to interpret the results with sufficient statistical
                                                                     power. Note that we found no significant differences between
                                                                     similar and expert advice in a separate analysis.
Figure 3. The pop-up screen in the unsolicited condition,            3.4.2. Subjective aspects
which presented energy-saving advice immediately after               We surveyed users on five subjective constructs: perceived
attitude calibration. Similar to Figure 2, each measure’s            recommendation quality, perceived system trust, threat to
name, costs, and kWh savings was listed. This example                negative face (i.e. whether one’s autonomy is affected), system
depicts advice explained through an expert source (NL:
                                                                     satisfaction, and choice satisfaction. For all these aspects, users
“expert advies”). Cropped off the top of the image is an
explanatory text in Dutch. Users could choose (NL: “Kies”)           were presented survey items on 7-point Likert scales and were
any of these measures if they wished to perform them.                asked to indicate to what extent they agreed with each item. The
                                                                     used questionnaire items are described in Table 1.
                                                                         As prescribed by Knijnenburg and Willemsen [19], we
3.3 Participants
                                                                     submitted all responses to a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA)
In total, 260 participants used our Saving Aid recommender           using ordinal dependent variables. Table 1 reports only three
system and finished the subsequent questionnaire. Among them,        user experience constructs, as we could not include the threat to
110 participants were recruited from the Jan Frederik Schouten       face construct in our analysis, for it had high cross loadings with
database of Eindhoven University of Technology, while others         all other aspects in our model. Moreover, we could not discern
were recruited through posts on social media (e.g. Twitter,          choice satisfaction from the system satisfaction construct, since
Facebook, etc.) Each participant entered a raffle in which they      it violated divergent validity [19]. The remaining constructs met
had a 20% probability to win €15. We omitted 8 participants from     the guidelines for convergence validity, as the average variance
analysis, as they either had finished the study in no more than 2    explained (AVE) for each construct was higher than 0.5, and had
minutes, or showed no variation in their answers on the              a good internal consistency (0.8 < α < 0.9) [11].
evaluation questionnaire.
   Eventually, we analyzed a sample of 252 participants (50.8%
                                                                          4    Results
female), which had a mean age of 31.9 years (SD = 16.0), and who
on average chose 8.9 measures (SD = 9.1). Furthermore,               We used Structural Equation Modeling (SEM) to organize all
approximately half of the sample was still a university student,     objective and subjective constructs, including relevant
who typically lived in shared apartments. Most of these students     interactions, into a path model. As prescribed by Knijnenburg
had an income that fell below €1000.                                 and Willemsen [19], a confirmatory factor analysis was
                                                                     performed first (cf. Table 1), after which we tested a fully
                                                                     saturated model and performed stepwise removal of non-
                                                                     significant relations. Figure 4 depicts the final path model, which
                                                                     had an excellent fit: χ2(113) = 131.625, p = 0.11, CFI = 0.995, TLI =
                                                                     0.996, RMSEA = 0.026, 90%-CI: [0.000,0.042].
Advice solicitation and social peers in energy recommender systems                         IntRS ’19, September 19, 2019, Copenhagen, Denmark

Table 1. Results of the confirmatory factory analysis on                 satisfaction showed that this effect was significant, mediated by
user experience. Items without loading were removed                      perceived trust: β = .151, 95%-CI: [0.004,0.298], p = .044.
from the final model, while the Choice Satisfaction and                      Second, Figure 4 also shows two different effects of users who
Threat to Negative Face aspects were excluded as they                    were in the solicited condition, but who did not solicit advice. If
violated divergent validity. The average variance                        such advice was offered using a socially-laden explanation
explained (AVE) and Cronbach’s Alpha of other aspects
                                                                         (either through expert advice or similar peers), it negatively
met the prescribed guidelines [11, 19].
                                                                         impacted the perceived recommendation quality compared to the
                                                                         unsolicited, non-social baseline: β = −1.18, p < 0.01. In contrast,
Aspect          Item                                             Loa‐    non-social explanations positively affected the perceived
                                                                 ding
                                                                         recommendation quality compared to the unsolicited baseline: β
Choice          I am happy with the measures I’ve chosen
Satisfaction    I know several measures that are better than             = .41, p < 0.05. Since non-soliciting users did not actually see a
                the ones I selected                                      list of highlighted energy-saving recommendations, it was
                I would recommend some of the chosen                     possible they evaluated the main interface instead, which
                measures to others                                       confounds a clear interpretation of this result.
                I am looking forward to implement the chosen
                measures
                The measures I’ve chosen fit me seamlessly

Perceived       I found the recommended measures to be            .813
Rec. Quality    attractive
                The recommended measures fitted my                .857
AVE: .68        preferences
Alpha: .82      The recommended measures were relevant to         .731
                me
                The Saving Aid proposed too many bad
                measures
                I did not like any of the measures

Perceived       I think that the Saving Aid was telling me the    .756
Sys. Trust      truth
                I expected the Saving Aid to be truthful          .670
AVE: .72        The Saving Aid was honest                         .747
Alpha: .88      The Saving Aid was sufficiently                          Figure 4. Structural Equation Model (SEM). The numbers
                knowledgeable to present advice                          on the arrows represent β-coefficients, standard errors are
                The Saving Aid had the best intentions            .624   denoted between brackets. The effects between the latent
                                                                         subjective constructs are standardized and can be
System          The Saving Aid has made me more aware of          .546   considered as correlations. Aspects are grouped by color:
satisfaction    my energy-saving behavior                                objective system aspects are purple, interaction aspects are
                I would like to use the Saving Aid more often     .663   blue, subjective aspects are green, and experience aspects
AVE: .70        I make better decisions using the Saving Aid      .710
                                                                         are orange. *** p < 0.001, ** p < 0.01, * p < 0.05.
Alpha: .88      The Saving Aid helps me to find appropriate       .620
                measures
                The Saving Aid allowed me to choose                      4.2 System evaluation and choice behavior
                measures easily
                                                                         Figure 4 depicts that users who had chosen a measure from
Threat to       The Saving Aid respected my autonomy and                 either a solicited or unsolicited recommendation list, perceived
Negative        the choices I made                                       the recommendation quality to be higher than those who did
Face            The Saving Aid did not impose anything on                not: β = .636, p < 0.001. Furthermore, Figure 4 shows two
                me
                I was free to choose any measure                         different pathways in which higher levels of recommendation
                I did not feel forced to take the Saving Aid’s           quality increase other subjective aspects. First, higher perceived
                advice                                                   recommendation quality positively affected perceived trust and,
                                                                         in turn, system satisfaction. Besides this mediated effect, Figure 4
4.1 Advice solicitation                                                  also shows a positive, direct path from recommendation quality
Figure 4 shows two effects of advice solicitation on how the             to system satisfaction, as well as a positive effect from users
recommender system and its advice were perceived. First, users           choosing a recommended measure to system satisfaction.
who had the option to solicit advice and actually did so (‘advice           Moreover, a positive evaluation of the system also led users to
solicitation’), reported higher levels of system trust than those        choose more energy-saving measures: β = .979, p < 0.05.
who were presented unsolicited advice: β = .414, p < 0.05. This          Although it could be possible to reverse this particular causal
suggested that users who asked for personalized advice                   direction (e.g. more choices led to higher satisfaction levels), the
perceived the system as more trustworthy, than those who                 current pathway was consistent with previous energy
immediately faced unsolicited advice, regardless of the social           recommender studies [18, 29], and led to the best model fit
source. A bootstrapped test of indirect effects towards system           statistics.
IntRS ’19, September 19, 2019, Copenhagen, Denmark                                                                                         A.D. Starke


     5    Discussion                                                   5.1 Limitations
As one of the first to do so, this study has applied the               Unfortunately, we could not relate our path model constructs to
phenomenon of face-to-face advice solicitation (asking for advice      choice satisfaction, for it violated divergent validity. However,
or not) to the HCI domain. We have investigated to what extent         the strong correlation between both system and choice
solicitation and social explanations of advice affect how a user       satisfaction suggests that trust would also have a positive effect
perceives and evaluates an energy recommender system.                  on how users evaluate their choices, which has shown to
    To do so, we have developed the ‘Saving Aid’ system, from          positively affect the probability that chosen measures are
which users could choose any number of energy-saving                   actually implemented and a user’s behavior is changed [29].
measures they would like to perform. It employs a simple                  Our findings are confounded by not all users soliciting advice
personalization algorithm, using the psychometric Rasch model,         in the ‘solicitation condition’. This has prevented us from
to generate appropriate attitude-tailored recommendations for          analyzing our original research design, which has now
each user. In this personalized advice context, we have found          overlooked any differences between specific social explanations,
small differences in how our energy recommender system is              either a similar peer or an expert. Although this should be
evaluated, based on whether advice is solicited by users or            further investigated in a follow-up study, the discrepancy
presented without being explicitly solicited.                          between social and non-social explanations of advice seems to be
    Our results show that users who have solicited personalized        an important finding in itself, which is also demonstrated by the
recommendations report higher system trust levels, compared to         interaction effect in our path model.
those who are presented unsolicited advice. It could be that users        Furthermore, in studies on face-to-face interactions, advice
wish to maintain control over which items they inspect, and that       solicitation is typically related to personality concepts as
a system that immediately determines which three items are             autonomy and threat to face. Due to a violation of divergent
appropriate, is perceived as less trustworthy. The fact that most      validity in our path model, we have been unable to test whether
recommender research does not consider whether some users              a violation of autonomy has decreased trust. Nonetheless, our
wish to solicit advice themselves, seems to be a missed                study still shows that changes in the recommender interface, due
opportunity, since higher trust levels in a recommender could          to advice solicitation and social explanations, lead to changes in
have important second-order effects. Indeed, in this study, we         system perception and choice behavior, and should be
find that users who report higher trust levels, also choose more       considered in future recommender designs.
energy-saving measures as a result of a positive user experience.
    Furthermore, we find mixed results for perceived                   ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
recommendation quality across social conditions. Users who             We thank Nena van As, Jochem Bek, and Boy Janissen for their
have the opportunity to solicit advice but do not do so, report        time and effort to conduct this study and discuss its implications.
lower levels of recommendation quality if advice is explained in       This work is part of the Research Talent program with project
terms of similar peers or experts, compared to users who face          number 406-14-088, which is financed by the Netherlands
unsolicited social advice. This effect reverses for non-social         Organization for Scientific Research (NWO). It was also made
explanations, as users who are presented the opportunity to            possible by funding from the Niels Stensen Fellowship.
solicit system advice report higher recommendation quality than
those who are presented unsolicited system advice. These               REFERENCES
findings either suggest that adding social explanations to             [1]    Allcott, H. 2011. Social norms and energy conservation. Journal of Public
unsolicited advice could mitigate a user’s feeling that the system            Economics. 95, 9–10 (Oct. 2011), 1082–1095.
is ‘butting in’, or that users who could solicit advice might not be          DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpubeco.2011.03.003.
                                                                       [2]    Bo Feng and MacGeorge, E.L. 2010. The Influences of Message and Source
interested in additional social advice after inspecting the main              Factors on Advice Outcomes. Communication Research. 37, 4 (Aug. 2010),
Saving Aid interface. However, these comparisons only apply to                553–575. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650210368258.
                                                                       [3]    Bonaccio, S. and Dalal, R.S. 2006. Advice taking and decision-making: An
those who have not clicked, which has probably increased the                  integrative literature review, and implications for the organizational
differences between conditions.                                               sciences. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 101, 2
                                                                              (Nov. 2006), 127–151. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2006.07.001.
    Our results resonate with earlier findings on inspectability       [4]    Bonhard, P. and Sasse, M.A. 2006. ’Knowing me, knowing you’ — Using
and control in social recommender systems [17], which has                     profiles and social networking to improve recommender systems. BT
shown to positively affect system evaluation. Furthermore, it                 Technology Journal. 24, 3 (Jul. 2006), 84–98.
                                                                              DOI:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10550-006-0080-3.
shows that findings from the advice-giving and taking literature       [5]    Caplan, S.E. and Samter, W. 1999. The role of facework in younger and
on face-to-face interactions do translate to the HCI context [e.g.            older adults’ evaluations of social support messages. Communication
                                                                              Quarterly. 47, 3 (1999), 245–264.
3, 12]. Even though the interactions in a typical recommender          [6]    Dalal, R.S. and Bonaccio, S. 2010. What types of advice do decision-makers
system are hardly anthropomorphic, the principles of trust,                   prefer? Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 112, 1 (May
quality, and possibly autonomy might also translate to                        2010), 11–23. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2009.11.007.
                                                                       [7]    Ekstrand, M.D. and Willemsen, M.C. 2016. Behaviorism is Not Enough:
personalized HCI. These factors may be particularly important in              Better Recommendations Through Listening to Users. Proceedings of the
domains where users wish to change their lifestyle, such as                   10th ACM Conference on Recommender Systems (New York, NY, USA, 2016),
                                                                              221–224.
health and energy conservation, which often rely on social proof       [8]    Fisher, J.D., Nadler, A. and Whitcher-Alagna, S. 1982. Recipient reactions
to achieve behavioral change [1, 27].                                         to aid. Psychological Bulletin. 91, 1 (1982), 27.
Advice solicitation and social peers in energy recommender systems                                          IntRS ’19, September 19, 2019, Copenhagen, Denmark

[9]    Gino, F. and Moore, D.A. 2007. Effects of task difficulty on use of advice.      [33]   Trattner, C. and Elsweiler, D. 2017. Food Recommender Systems:
       Journal of Behavioral Decision Making. 20, 1 (Jan. 2007), 21–35.                        Important Contributions, Challenges and Future Research Directions.
       DOI:https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.539.                                                    arXiv:1711.02760 [cs]. (Nov. 2017).
[10]   Gino, F., Shang, J. and Croson, R. 2009. The impact of information from          [34]   Urban, J. and Ščasný, M. 2016. Structure of Domestic Energy Saving: How
       similar or different advisors on judgment. Organizational Behavior and                  Many Dimensions? Environment and Behavior. 48, 3 (Apr. 2016), 454–481.
       Human Decision Processes. 108, 2 (Mar. 2009), 287–302.                                  DOI:https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916514547081.
       DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2008.08.002.                                 [35]   Yaniv, I., Choshen-Hillel, S. and Milyavsky, M. 2011. Receiving advice on
[11]   Gliem, J.A. and Gliem, R.R. 2003. Calculating, Interpreting, And Reporting              matters of taste: Similarity, majority influence, and taste discrimination.
       Cronbach’s Alpha Reliability Coefficient For Likert-Type Scales. (2003).                Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 115, 1 (May 2011),
[12]   Goldsmith, D.J. 2000. Soliciting advice: The role of sequential placement in            111–120. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2010.11.006.
       mitigating face threat. Communication Monographs. 67, 1 (Mar. 2000), 1–19.
       DOI:https://doi.org/10.1080/03637750009376492.
[13]   Goldsmith, D.J. and Fitch, K. 1997. The Normative Context of Advice as
       Social Support. Human Communication Research. 23, 4 (Jun. 1997), 454–476.
       DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.1997.tb00406.x.
[14]   Harvey, N. and Fischer, I. 1997. Taking Advice: Accepting Help, Improving
       Judgment, and Sharing Responsibility. Organizational Behavior and Human
       Decision Processes. 70, 2 (May 1997), 117–133.
       DOI:https://doi.org/10.1006/obhd.1997.2697.
[15]   Herlocker, J.L., Konstan, J.A., Terveen, L.G. and Riedl, J.T. 2004. Evaluating
       Collaborative Filtering Recommender Systems. ACM Trans. Inf. Syst. 22, 1
       (Jan. 2004), 5–53. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1145/963770.963772.
[16]   Kaiser, F.G., Byrka, K. and Hartig, T. 2010. Reviving Campbell’s Paradigm
       for Attitude Research. Personality and Social Psychology Review. 14, 4 (Nov.
       2010), 351–367. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868310366452.
[17]   Knijnenburg, B.P., Bostandjiev, S., O’Donovan, J. and Kobsa, A. 2012.
       Inspectability and control in social recommenders. Proceedings of the sixth
       ACM conference on Recommender systems (2012), 43–50.
[18]   Knijnenburg, B.P., Willemsen, M. and Broeders, R. 2014. Smart
       sustainability through system satisfaction: Tailored preference elicitation
       for energy-saving recommenders. Proceedings 20th Americas Conference on
       Information Systems (AMCIS 2014): Smart Sustainability: The Information
       Systems Opportunity (Savannah, Georgia, United States, 2014).
[19]   Knijnenburg, B.P. and Willemsen, M.C. 2015. Evaluating recommender
       systems with user experiments. Recommender Systems Handbook. Springer.
       309–352.
[20]   Koestner, R., Gingras, I., Abutaa, R., Losier, G.F., DiDio, L. and Gagné, M.
       1999. To Follow Expert Advice When Making a Decision: An Examination
       of Reactive Versus Reflective Autonomy. Journal of Personality. 67, 5
       (1999), 851–872. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-6494.00075.
[21]   Meyer, E. 2014. The culture map: Breaking through the invisible boundaries
       of global business. Public Affairs.
[22]   Murray, H.A. 1938. Explorations in personality: A clinical and
       experimental study of fifty men of college age. (1938).
[23]   Nolan, J.M., Schultz, P.W., Cialdini, R.B., Goldstein, N.J. and Griskevicius,
       V. 2008. Normative social influence is underdetected. Personality and social
       psychology bulletin. 34, 7 (2008), 913–923.
[24]   Schäfer, H., Hors-Fraile, S., Karumur, R.P., Calero Valdez, A., Said, A.,
       Torkamaan, H., Ulmer, T. and Trattner, C. 2017. Towards Health (Aware)
       Recommender Systems. Proceedings of the 2017 International Conference on
       Digital Health (New York, NY, USA, 2017), 157–161.
[25]   Schäfer, H. and Willemsen, M.C. 2019. Rasch-based Tailored Goals for
       Nutrition Assistance Systems. Proceedings of the 24th International
       Conference on Intelligent User Interfaces (New York, NY, USA, 2019), 18–29.
[26]   Schrah, G.E., Dalal, R.S. and Sniezek, J.A. 2006. No decision-maker is an
       Island: integrating expert advice with information acquisition. Journal of
       Behavioral Decision Making. 19, 1 (Jan. 2006), 43–60.
       DOI:https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.514.
[27]   Schultz, P.W., Nolan, J.M., Cialdini, R.B., Goldstein, N.J. and Griskevicius,
       V. 2007. The constructive, destructive, and reconstructive power of social
       norms. Psychological science. 18, 5 (2007), 429–434.
[28]   Sharma, A. and Cosley, D. 2013. Do social explanations work?: studying
       and modeling the effects of social explanations in recommender systems.
       Proceedings of the 22nd international conference on World Wide Web (2013),
       1133–1144.
[29]   Starke, A., Willemsen, M. and Snijders, C. 2017. Effective User Interface
       Designs to Increase Energy-efficient Behavior in a Rasch-based Energy
       Recommender System. Proceedings of the Eleventh ACM Conference on
       Recommender Systems (New York, NY, USA, 2017), 65–73.
[30]   Starke, A., Willemsen, M.C. and Snijders, C. 2015. Saving Energy in 1-D:
       Tailoring Energy-saving Advice Using a Rasch-based Energy
       Recommender System. DMRS (2015), 5–8.
[31]   Starke, A.D. 2019. Supporting energy-efficient choices using Rasch-based
       recommender interfaces. (2019).
[32]   Tomkins, S., Isley, S. and Getoor, L. 2018. Sustainability at Scale: Towards
       Bridging the Intention-Behavior Gap with Sustainable Recommendations.
       Proceedings of the 12th ACM Conference on Recommender Systems
       (Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada, 2018), 214–218.