=Paper= {{Paper |id=Vol-2524/paper14 |storemode=property |title=Learning from a scientific discourse through video lessons: when gestures can’t help |pdfUrl=https://ceur-ws.org/Vol-2524/paper14.pdf |volume=Vol-2524 |authors=Francesco Ianì,Alessandro Lombardo,Monica Bucciarelli |dblpUrl=https://dblp.org/rec/conf/psychobit/IaniLB19 }} ==Learning from a scientific discourse through video lessons: when gestures can’t help== https://ceur-ws.org/Vol-2524/paper14.pdf
      Learning from a Scientific Discourse through Video
             Lessons: When Gestures can’t help 1

          Francesco Ianì[1] Alessandro Lombardo[2] and Monica Bucciarelli[1-3]
                1 Dipartimento di Psicologia, Università di Torino, Torino, Italy
                                    2 Choralia, Milano, Italy
        3 Centro di Logica, Linguaggio, e Cognizione, Università di Torino, Torino, Italy

                               francesco.iani@unito.it




        Abstract. The classical literature on co-speech gestures has proved their beneficial role
        in comprehension and learning from discourse. However, while the role of gestures in
        narrative discourse comprehension has been widely explored, their role in scientific
        discourse comprehension has been neglected and most of the literature on learning
        science is concerned with gestures accompanying single scientific concepts. Since
        instruction done by video can exploit the power of gestures, our twofold aim was to
        explore the effect of gestures accompanying a scientific discourse delivered through
        video. In three experiments we ascertained whether learning from scientific discourse
        through videos benefits from gestures, observed or produced. The results have revealed
        that comprehension and learning from a scientific discourse do not improve when the
        teacher gestures compared to when the teacher does not gesture (Experiments 1 and 2)
        and that learner’s gestures, as compared to teacher’s gestures, can worsen comprehension
        and learning (Experiments 2 and 3). These results have implications for technology
        enhanced learning.

        Keywords: Learning, Scientific Discourse, Video Lessons, Gestures.


1       Introduction
Speakers’ co-speech gestures favor deep discourse comprehension in listeners (Cutica
& Bucciarelli, 2015). Many researchers have argued that the successful comprehension
of a discourse is tantamount to the construction of a coherent mental model (Zwaan &
Radvansky, 1998), and according to different theoretical frameworks, such
representations are referred to as “situation model” (van Dijk & Kintsch, 1983) or
“mental model” (Johnson-Laird, 1983). Following the tenets of the mental model
theory, Bucciarelli (2007) and Cutica and Bucciarelli (2008; 2011) advanced a mental
model account for the cognitive change produced by gestures: gestures, whether
observed or produced, favor the construction of a mental model of the discourse they
accompany. Since mental models are discrete representations in nature, the information
conveyed by co-speech gestures, also represented in a non-discrete format, can be easily

1
    Copyright © 2019 for this paper by its authors. Use permitted under Creative
     Commons License Attribution 4.0 International (CC BY 4.0)
2




incorporated into the discourse mental model. In line with these assumptions, also
learners benefit from gestures production in learning contexts (Novack &
GoldinMeadow, 2015).
    Relevant to the present investigation, teachers communicate in classroom settings
by using gestures (Alibali et al., 2014) and students can detect conceptual information
expressed in those gestures (Kelly & Church, 1998) and can benefit from them
(Koumoutsakis et al., 2016). Teacher’s gestures have been proved beneficial for
learning scientific concepts in instruction done live. For example, a study has revealed
improved performance on a posttest after a lesson on the notion of conservation
accompanied by representational gestures compared to a lesson that did not contain
gestures (Church et al., 2004). Similar results have been obtained by studies concerned
with learning math concepts (Valenzo et al., 2003). Also learners’ gestures can facilitate
learning (Ianì, Cutica & Bucciarelli, 2016). Studies have shown that fourth-grade
children who produced gestures during instruction on a math task were more likely to
retain and generalize the knowledge they gained, than children who did not gesture
(Cook & Goldin-Meadow, 2006). Other studies have revealed that when instructed to
gesture while explaining math problems, children added new problem-solving
strategies to their repertoire and remembered more from a subsequent lesson from the
teacher (Broaders et al., 2007).
    Instruction done on video can be as effective as done live (Koumoutsakis et al.,
2016) and is seen as a viable alternative to face-to-face teaching for a host of reasons
(Allen & Seaman, 2010). Relevant to the aim of our investigation, instruction by video
can exploit the power of gestures. For example, studies have revealed that adult learners
benefit from videos of teachers gesturing respect to pictures while teaching about gear
movement (Carlson et al., 2014) or from videos of teachers pointing at slides while
teaching a complex statistical concepts (Rueckert et al., 2017). As for instruction done
on video and involving learner’s gestures, at our knowledge there are no studies in the
literature.
    In general, the studies concerning gestures in instruction done live or by video have
been concerned with the role of gestures in learning single scientific concepts and it is
not obvious that their findings can be extended to learning from a scientific discourse
featuring several related concepts. The twofold aim of our investigation was to ascertain
whether learning from scientific discourse through videos benefits from gestures,
observed or produced. We present three experiments on the role of gestures in learning
from science video lessons. In Experiment 1, the participants watched two video
lessons: in one video the teacher accompanied the discourse with gestures and in the
other video he proffered the discourse while staying still. At recall, the participants
recalled few concepts; a possible explanation is the transient high number of concepts.
Experiments 2 and 3 used the same scientific lessons but segmented in parts for two
reasons. First, the procedure allowed the participants more time to elaborate the
information. Second, we could manipulate the variable gesture (no gesture, teacher’s
gesture, learner’s gesture) and gain more in-depth understanding of the effect of
gestures on learning from scientific discourse through video lessons.
                                                                                          3




2      Does Learning from Scientific Video Lessons benefit from
       Gestures?
A peculiarity of learning from discourse is the transient nature of the information that
posits a noteworthy cognitive load on learners. Also instruction by video is transient in
that requires learners to keep the disappeared information in mind in order to
comprehend the next piece of information (Ayres & Paas, 2007) and this task is
demanding for working memory, which has a limited capacity (Cowan, 2001).
Scientific discourse, in particular, has a difficulty that determines an intrinsic load; the
higher the number of interacting information elements, the more difficult the material
is for the learner and the higher the intrinsic load (Sweller, 1994). Difficulty also
depends on learner expertise: with increasing expertise more information elements are
combined into schemata, which reduces the intrinsic load of a task.
    The participants in our experiment encountered two scientific discourses with a high
intrinsic load: they concern topics unknown to the participants (the airplane flying and
the sound propagation) and feature a domain-specific terminology. In Experiment 1, the
discourses were presented in their normal flow, without interruptions, whereas in
Experiments 2 and 3 they were segmented. The rationale was to ascertain whether
presenting information in pieces rather than as a continuous stream makes videos more
effective for learning (see Spanjers et al., 2012, for this sort of evidence).


2.1    Experiment 1: Science Video Lessons: Teacher’s Gestures don’t help
The task of the participants in the experiment was watching two videos in which an
actor/teacher proffered a scientific discourse; one of the discourse was accompanied by
gestures and the other was not. Aim of the experiment was to ascertain whether
comprehension and memory was better for the discourse accompanied by gestures
(observed-gesture) than for the discourse proffered without gesturing (no-gesture).

Method

Participants. Twenty-eight students from Università di Torino (14 males and 14
females; mean age = 23.9 years, SD = 1.6 years) voluntarily took part in the experiment
in exchange of course credits and after informed consent.

Material and procedures. The experimental material consisted in two scientific
discourses, one concerning the airplane flight (hereafter, Airplane) and the other
concerning the nature of sound (hereafter, Sound). The texts were presented in the form
of an oral discourse by an actor, each in two conditions: in the observed-gesture
condition the actor accompanies the discourse with gestures whereas in the no-gesture
condition the actor stays still while proffering the discourse. The actor (the second
author, a professional actor with a degree in computer science and qualified to teach in
high school) had been instructed to study the two texts before the recording of the
4




videos, and to plan the gestures to produce along with the speech in the observedgesture
condition.
    Each participant encountered both discourses, one in the observed-gesture condition
and the other in the no-gesture condition. Half of the participants dealt first with the
observed-gesture condition, and half with the no-gesture condition; in each group, half
of the participants encountered first the Airplane discourse, and half of them the Sound
discourse. In each condition, the participants attended each discourse twice, and then
were invited to recollect as much information as they could. All of the participants were
video-recorded.
    To code the results, each discourse was divided into 52 semantic units,
corresponding to as many main concepts that the learner could recall. Each concept (i.e.,
semantic unit) recalled by the participants was evaluated by two independent judges
according to the following coding schema:
    • Correct recollection: a semantic unit recollected literally or as a paraphrase.
    • Discourse-based inference: a recollection in which the participant gave explicit
      information that was originally implicit in the semantic unit.
    • Elaborative inference: a semantic unit recollected with the addition of plausible
      details.
    • Erroneous recollection: a recollection with a meaning that was inconsistent with
      the semantic unit.
Consider, for instance, the following semantic unit in the Airplane discourse: “The air
divides as it hits the front of the wing”. According to the coding schema, the statement
“(The air) is divided in to two” was a correct recollection; the statement “(The air)
coming on the wing separates and for this there are two different speeds” was a
discourse-based inference (because it refers to a causal consequent). Now consider the
following semantic unit in the discourse: “This difference creates what is known as an
aerofoil”, according to the coding schema, the statement “the wing profile is the shape
of the wing” was an elaborative inference and the statement “He goes to imagine what
a wing profile is” was an erroneous recollection.

Results. The two independent judges coded the participants’ recollections individually.
The judges reached a significant level of agreement on their first judgments (agreed on
79.7% of the coding, Cohen’s K = .56, p < .001). For the final score, the judges
discussed each item on which they disagreed, until reaching a full agreement.
    Table 1 shows the mean types of recollections in the two conditions of the
experiment. Results revealed no differences across the two conditions in the number of
erroneous recollections (t(27)=1.22, p = .23), as well in the number of elaborative
inferences (t(27)=1.66, p = .11) and in the number of discoursed based inferences
(t(27)=1.39, p = .18). Crucially, we did not detect a significant difference across the two
conditions in the number of correct recollections (t(27)=0.21, p = .84).
    If the gestures of the speaker were not effective because there was not enough time
to elaborate them along with the numerous scientific concepts, then gestures, compared
to no gestures, should favor comprehension and learning if the time to process them
along with speech is increased. We devised Experiment 2, in which the scientific
discourses were presented segmented. Further, in Experiment 2 we added a condition
                                                                                               5




in which listeners were invited to produce their own gestures; since studies in the
literature suggest that gestures production can be more effective than gesture
observation (e.g., Hornstein & Mulligan, 2004), it is possible that also in case of
learning from a scientific discourse gestures production is more effective than gestures
observation. Another possibility is that, given the high number of related scientific
concepts, learning from a segmented discourse, still transient in nature, does not benefit
from learners’ gestures.


 Table 1. Mean types of recollections (and standard deviations in paretheses) in the no-gesture
                     and observed-gesture conditions in Experiment 1.
                  Correct            Discourse-based            Elaborative           Errors
Condition      recollections             inferences             inferences
                     13.80                   0.64                   0.46               0.68
No-gesture           (4.90)                 (0.95)                 (0.64)             (1.00)

Observed-            14.00                   1.00                   0.21
                                                                                       1.00
gesture              (5.20)                 (1.20)                 (0.50)
                                                                                      (1.12)




2.2     Experiment 2: Science Video Lessons: the Learner’s Gestures worsen
        Comprehension and Learning
Each participant in the experiment watched one video in which the actor proffered a
discourse. Each video was segmented so that between the introduction of a concept and
another one there was a pause. The experiment featured three experimental conditions
and each participant was randomly assigned to one of the three conditions: the discourse
was proffered by the actor while staying still and the participants attended the video
while staying still (no-gesture condition); the actor accompanied the discourse with
gestures and the participants stayed still (observed-gesture condition); the actor stayed
still and the participants were invited to gesture, in the pause, so to represent the concept
just introduced by the actor (produced-gesture condition).


Method

Participants. Thirty students from Università di Torino (3 males and 27 females; mean
age = 22.5 years, SD = 1.4 years) voluntarily took part in the experiment in exchange
of course credits and after informed consent. None of them had taken part in Experiment
1.

Material and procedures. The experimental material consisted in the two scientific
discourses of Experiment 1. There were three experimental conditions and each
participant was randomly assigned to one of the three: no-gesture, observed-gesture and
 6




produced-gesture conditions. In each condition, half of the participants encountered the
Airplane discourse and half of them the Sound discourse. Participants in each
experimental condition attended the discourse twice, and then were invited to recollect
as much information as they could. All of the participants were video-recorded.
Participants’ recollections were coded as in Experiment 1 by two independent judges.

Results. The two independent judges coded the participants’ recollections individually.
The judges reached a significant level of agreement on their first judgments (agreed on
89.5% of the coding, Cohen’s K = .70, p < .001). For the final score, the judges
discussed each item on which they disagreed, until reaching a full agreement.
     Table 2 shows the mean types of recollections in the three conditions of the
experiment. There was a statistically significant difference between groups in terms of
correct recollections as determined by one-way ANOVA (F(2,27) = 8.12, p = .002,
η p 2=.38). A Bonferroni post hoc test revealed that the number of correct recollections
was statistically significantly lower in the produced-gesture group compared to the
nogesture and the observed-gesture groups (for both comparison, p = .005). There was
no statistically significant difference between the no-gesture and the produced-gesture
groups (p = 1). No difference between groups has been detected in terms of
discoursebased inferences (F(2,27) = 0.46, p = .64), elaborative inferences (F(2,27) =
0.06, p = .94), and erroneous recollections (F(2,27) = 1.00, p = .38).


 Table 2. Mean types of recollections (and standard deviations in parentheses) in the no-gesture,
             observed-gesture and produced-gesture conditions in Experiment 2.
                   Correct             Discourse-based           Elaborative            Errors
Condition       recollections             inferences              inferences
No- gesture         21.60                    1.00                    0.30                1.00
                   (6.79)                   (1.41)                  (0.48)              (1.63)


Observed             21.70                                           0.30                1.90
                                              1.40
gesture              (8.31)                                         (0.97)              (1.25)
                                             (1.17)

Produced             11.30                    0.90                   0.40                1.00
gesture              (4.00)                  (1.10)                  (.97)              (1.25)



    The results of Experiment 2, when compared with those of Experiment 1, suggest
that participants in Experiment 1 recalled fewer semantic units because they had
difficulty in following the flow of the oral discourse. Participants in Experiment 2 had
more time to invest in learning, but evidently not enough to benefit from gestures.
Indeed, the results of Experiment 2 have revealed that gestures do not favor learning
from a segmented scientific discourse. These results contrast with those of studies in
the literature concerning learning from scientific texts; they suggest that environments
that allow learner control in terms of going back and forward in the text and in the time
                                                                                       7




invested in learning can exploit the beneficial effect of gestures. For example, studies
have revealed that learning from scientific texts benefits from gestures production in
learner-paced learning environments (Cutica & Bucciarelli, 2013; Cutica et al., 2014).
As for the effectiveness of gestures when observed as compared to when produced, the
results of Experiment 2 reveal that gesture production worsen learning. Experiment 3
was a within-subjects study whose aim was to investigate more in-depth the effects of
gestures observation and gestures production in learning from scientific video lessons.

2.3    Experiment 3: Science Video Lessons: A more in depth investigation into
       the effect of Teacher’s and Learner’s gestures
The experiment was a replication of Experiment 2, but the design was within and there
were only two conditions: observed-gesture and produced-gesture.


Method

Participants. Twenty students from Università di Torino (4 males and 16 females; mean
age = 22.6 years, SD = 4.8 years) voluntarily took part in the experiment in exchange
of course credits and after informed consent. None of them had taken part in
Experiments 1 and 2.

Material and procedures. The experimental material was the same as for Experiments
1 and 2. There were two experimental conditions: observed-gesture and
producedgesture. Each participant dealt with the two conditions: half of them
encountered the Airplane discourse in the observed-gesture condition and the Sound
discourse in the produced-gesture condition, and half of them encountered the Sound
discourse in the observed-gesture condition and the Airplane discourse in the produced-
gesture condition. The experimental procedures and the coding of results was also the
same as for the previous experiments.

Results. Two independent judges coded the participants’ recollections individually.
The judges reached a significant level of agreement on their first judgments (agreed on
94.6% of the coding, Cohen’s K = .75, p < .001). For the final score, the judges
discussed each item on which they disagreed, until reaching a full agreement.
   Table 3 shows the mean types of recollections in the two conditions of the
experiment. We detected a significant difference across the two conditions in terms of
correct recollections (t(19) = 3.01, p = .007, d’= 0.676). No difference was detected for
elaborative inferences (t(27)=1.00, p = .33), discoursed based inferences (t(27)=0.28, p
= .82) and erroneous recollections (t(27)=0.34, p = .74).
 8




      Table 3. Mean types of recollections (and standard deviations in parentheses) in the
              observedgesture and produced-gesture conditions in Experiment 3.
                     Correct          Discourse-based           Elaborative            Errors
Condition         recollections          inferences             inferences
Observedgesture      21.20                   0.70                   0.05                0.95
                     (6.18)                 (1.17)                 (0.22)              (1.28)
Producedgesture      16.20                   0.60                   0.35                0.80
                     (6.43)                 (1.27)                 (1.57)              (1.32)



3       Discussion and Conclusions
The aim of the present investigation was to explore the effect of gestures accompanying
a scientific discourse delivered through video. In three experiments, we ascertained
whether learning from scientific discourse through videos benefits from gestures,
observed or produced. Globally considered, the results of Experiments 1 and 2 have
revealed that comprehension and learning from a scientific discourse do not improve
when the teacher gestures compared to when the teacher does not gesture, and the
results of Experiment 2 and 3 have revealed that the learner’s gestures, as compared to
the teacher’s gestures, can worsen comprehension and learning.
    A tentative explanation for this global pattern of result is that since science lessons
involve knowledge of a high specialized vocabulary, when learners lack this
knowledge, they might struggle to construct the discourse mental model also when the
discourse is accompanied by gestures. Indeed, within a mental model perspective, a text
can be represented at three levels: the surface representation, the textbase
representation, and the mental model representation. The surface representation consists
of the verbatim words and clauses extracted from the discourse. At the textbase level,
the meanings of words and clauses are processed and subsequently stored in the
listener’s memory. A mental model representation is a coherent and non-linguistic
mental representation of the ‘state-of-affairs’ described in a discourse, whose
precondition is the processing of the meanings of words.
    But why gestures produced as compared to gestures observed do worsen
comprehension and learning? A possible explanation is that the high specialized
language involved in our scientific discourse posited a high cognitive load on
participants and the worry to plan and produce gestures was an extra-demand for
participants in the produced-gesture condition as compared to observed-gesture
condition.
    A significant difference between Experiment 1 and Experiments 2 and 3 is that the
latter used segmented video lessons. Relevant for the present investigation, segmented
video lessons, compared to video lessons presented in their natural flow, favored
comprehension and learning. This result is in line with those of studies revealing that
segmenting information, that is presenting them in pieces rather than as a continuous
stream, makes videos more effective for learning in that have a beneficial effect on
cognitive load and learning for novices. Pauses inserted between the segments may give
                                                                                        9




learners extra time to perform necessary cognitive processes (Spanjers et al., 2012). By
segmenting the discourse and providing pauses between a segment and another,
participants could have the time to apply more elaborate discourse-processing
strategies.
    In conclusion, the implications of the results of the three experiments for technology
enhanced learning are that video lessons in which the teacher gestures seem to have no
advantage on video lessons in which the teacher does not gestures, but segmented video
lessons should be preferred to video lessons delivered in their plain flow.


References
Alibali, M. W., Nathan, M. J., Wolfgram, M. S., Church, R. B., Jacobs, S. A., Johnson
   Martinez, C., … Knuth, E. J. (2014). How teachers link ideas in mathematics
   instruction using speech and gesture: A corpus analysis. Cognition and Instruction,
   32(1), 65–100.
Allen, I. E., & Seaman, J. (2010). Learning on demand: Online education in the United
   States, 2009. Newburyport, MA: The Sloan Consortium.
Ayres, P., & Paas, F. (2007). Can the cognitive load approach make instructional
   animations more effective? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 21, 811-820.
Broaders, S. C., Cook, S. W., Mitchell, Z., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2007). Making
   children gesture brings out implicit knowledge and leads to learning. Journal of
   Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 539-550.
Bucciarelli, M. (2007). How the construction of mental models improves learning. Mind
   & Society, 1(6), 67-89.
Carlson, C., Jacobs, A. A., Perry & Brecknridge Church, R. (2014). The effect of
   gestured instruction on the learning of physical causality problems. Gesture, 14(1),
   26-45.
Church, R. B., Ayman-Nolley, S., & Mahootian, S. (2004). The role of gesture in
   bilingual education: Does gesture enhance learning? International Journal of
   Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 7(4), 303-319.
Cook, S.W., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2006). The role of gestures in learning: Do children
   use their hands to change their minds? Journal of Cognition and Development, 7,
   211–232.
Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of
   mental storage capacity. Behavioral and Brain Science, 24, 87-185.
Cutica, I., & Bucciarelli, M. (2008). The deep versus the shallow: Effects of co-speech
   gestures in learning from discourse. Cognitive Science, 32, 921-935.
Cutica, I. & Bucciarelli, M. (2011). “The more you gesture, the less I gesture”:
   Cospeech gestures as a measure of mental model quality. Journal of Nonverbal
   Behavior, 35, 173-187.
Cutica, I. & Bucciarelli, M. (2013). Cognitive change in learning from text: Gesturing
   enhances the construction of the text mental model. Journal of Cognitive
   Psychology, 25,2, 201-209.
10




Cutica, I. & Bucciarelli, M. (2015). Non-determinism in the uptake of gestural
    information. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 39, 289-315
Cutica, I., Ianì, F., & Bucciarelli, M. (2014). Learning from text benefits from
    enactment. Memory & Cognition, 42, 1026-1037.
Hornstein, S. L., & Mulligan, N. W. (2004). Memory for actions: Enactment and source
    memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(2), 367-372.
Ianì, F., Cutica, I., & Bucciarelli, M. (2016). Timing of gestures: Gestures anticipating
    or simultaneous with speech as indexes of text comprehension in children and
    adults. Cognitive Science, 41(6), 1549-1566.
Johnson-Laird, P.N. (1983). Mental models: Towards a cognitive science of language,
    and consciousness. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.
Kelly, S. & Church, R.B. (1998). A comparison between children’s and adults’ ability
    to detect conceptual information conveyed through representational gestures. Child
    Development, 69, 85-93.
Koumoutsakis, T., Breckindridge Church, R., Alibali, M. W., Singer, M., &
    AymanNolley, S. (2016). Gesture in instruction: Evidence from live and video
    lessons. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 40, 301-315.
Novack, M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2015). Learning from gesture: How our hands
    change our minds. Educational Psychology Review, 27(3), 405-412.
Rueckert, L., Breckinridge Church, R., Avila, A., & Trejo, T. (2017). Gesture enhances
    learning of a complex statistical concept. Cognitive Research: Principles and
    Implications, 2:2.
Spanjers, I. A. E., Van Gog, T., Wouters, P., & Van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2012).
    Explaining the segmentation effect in learning from animations: The role of pausing
    and temporal cueing. Computers & Education, 59, 274-280.
Sweller, J. (1994). Cognitive load theory, learning difficulty, and instructional design.
    Learning and Instruction, 4, 295-312.
Valenzano, L, Alibali, M. W., & Klatzky, R. (2013). Teachers’ gestures facilitate
students’ learning: A lesson in symmetry. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 28,
187-204. van Dijk, I.A., & Kintsch, W. (1983). Strategies of discourse comprehension.
New York: Academic Press.
Zwaan, R. A., & Radvansky G. A. (1998). Situation models in language comprehension
    and memory. Psychological Bulletin, 123(2), 162-185.